Does an auditory distractor sequence affect self-paced tapping?

This study investigated whether an auditory distractor (D) sequence affects the timing of self-paced finger tapping. To begin with, Experiment 1 replicated earlier findings by showing that, when taps are synchronized with an isochronous auditory target (T) sequence, an isochronous D sequence of different tempo and pitch systematically modulates the tap timing. The extent of the modulation depended on the relative intensity of the T and D tones, but not on their pitch distance. Experiment 2 then used a synchronization-continuation paradigm in which D sequences of different tempi were introduced only during continuation tapping. Although the D sequences rarely captured the taps completely, they did increase the tapping variability and deviations from the correct tempo. Furthermore, they eliminated the negative correlation between successive inter-tap intervals and led to intermittent phase locking when the tapping period was close to the period of the D sequence. These distractor effects occurred regardless of whether or not the taps generated auditory feedback tones. The distractor effects thus depend neither on the intention to synchronize with a T sequence nor on the simultaneous perception of two auditory sequences. Rather, they seem to reflect a basic attraction of rhythmic movement to auditory rhythms.

[1]  Paul Bertelson,et al.  Temporal ventriloquism: crossmodal interaction on the time dimension. 2. Evidence from sensorimotor synchronization. , 2003, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[2]  Peter E. Keller,et al.  Adaptation to tempo changes in sensorimotor synchronization: Effects of intention, attention, and awareness , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[3]  B H Repp,et al.  Compensation for subliminal timing perturbations in perceptual-motor synchronization , 2000, Psychological research.

[4]  A. Kingstone,et al.  Auditory capture of vision: examining temporal ventriloquism. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[5]  Edward W. Large,et al.  Tracking simple and complex sequences , 2002, Psychological research.

[6]  Dirk Vorberg,et al.  Linear phase-correction in synchronization: predictions, parameter estimation, and simulations , 2002 .

[7]  B. Repp On the nature of phase attraction in sensorimotor synchronization with interleaved auditory sequences. , 2004, Human movement science.

[8]  A Semjen,et al.  Timing precision in continuation and synchronization tapping , 2000, Psychological research.

[9]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  B H Repp,et al.  Phase correction, phase resetting, and phase shifts after subliminal timing perturbations in sensorimotor synchronization. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  D Hary,et al.  Temporal tracking and synchronization strategies. , 1985, Human neurobiology.

[12]  Bruno H. Repp,et al.  Auditory dominance in temporal processing: new evidence from synchronization with simultaneous visual and auditory sequences. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  B H Repp,et al.  Processes underlying adaptation to tempo changes in sensorimotor synchronization. , 2001, Human movement science.

[14]  R Fendrich,et al.  The temporal cross-capture of audition and vision , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[15]  L. T. Stevens ON THE TIME-SENSE , 1886 .

[16]  A. Kristofferson,et al.  The timing of interresponse intervals , 1973 .

[17]  Bruno H Repp,et al.  Phase correction in sensorimotor synchronization: nonlinearities in voluntary and involuntary responses to perturbations. , 2002, Human movement science.

[18]  Bruno H Repp,et al.  Phase attraction in sensorimotor synchronization with auditory sequences: effects of single and periodic distractors on synchronization accuracy. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[19]  Pressing,et al.  Error Correction Processes in Temporal Pattern Production. , 1998, Journal of mathematical psychology.

[20]  B. Repp,et al.  Rhythmic movement is attracted more strongly to auditory than to visual rhythms , 2004, Psychological research.

[21]  Jürgen Kurths,et al.  Synchronization - A Universal Concept in Nonlinear Sciences , 2001, Cambridge Nonlinear Science Series.

[22]  Jirí Mates,et al.  A model of synchronization of motor acts to a stimulus sequence , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[23]  Bruno H. Repp Automaticity and voluntary control of phase correction following event onset shifts in sensorimotor synchronization. , 2002 .

[24]  R Todd Ogden,et al.  Adding drift to the decomposition of simple isochronous tapping: an extension of the Wing-Kristofferson model. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  A. Opstal Dynamic Patterns: The Self-Organization of Brain and Behavior , 1995 .

[26]  Michael J. Richardson,et al.  Effects of visual and verbal interaction on unintentional interpersonal coordination. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  G. Recanzone Auditory influences on visual temporal rate perception. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  R. Schmidt,et al.  Evaluating the Dynamics of Unintended Interpersonal Coordination , 1997 .

[29]  R. Remington,et al.  Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: evidence for two forms of attentional capture. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  D. Hary,et al.  Synchronizing human movement with an external clock source , 1987, Biological Cybernetics.

[31]  B H Repp,et al.  Acoustics, perception, and production of legato articulation on a computer-controlled grand piano. , 1997, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.