Centrophobism/thigmotaxis, a new role for the mushroom bodies in Drosophila.

Insects, like vertebrates, exhibit spatially complex locomotor activity patterns when foraging or navigating. Open field studies recently showed that Drosophila avoids central zones and stays at the periphery, an effect that can be interpreted as centrophobism and/or thigmotaxis. In this study, we further characterized this phenomenon and studied the responsible underlying neural mechanisms. The implication of the Drosophila mushroom bodies (MBs) in olfactory learning and memory processes is well documented. In an open field situation in which fly locomotor activity is recorded by video tracking, we show that center avoidance is greatly diminished in flies with hydroxyurea-ablated MBs, suggesting a new role for these structures. Furthermore, the temperature-sensitive allele of the dynamin gene shibire was expressed in various enhancer-trap P[GAL4] lines, disrupting synaptic transmission in different MB lobes. Specifically blocking the gamma lobes alters centrophobism/thigmotaxis while blocking the alpha/beta lobes does not, suggesting a functional specialization of MB lobes. Drosophila may serve as a new model system for elucidating the genetic and neural bases of such complex phenomena as centrophobism/thigmotaxis.

[1]  Jean-René Martin A portrait of locomotor behaviour in Drosophila determined by a video-tracking paradigm , 2004, Behavioural Processes.

[2]  K. Götz,et al.  Centrophobism inDrosophila melanogaster , 1985, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[3]  J. O’Keefe,et al.  Hippocampal place units in the freely moving rat: Why they fire where they fire , 1978, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  M. Heisenberg,et al.  Neuronal architecture of the central complex in Drosophila melanogaster , 2004, Cell and Tissue Research.

[5]  K. Schildberger,et al.  Multimodal interneurons in the cricket brain: properties of identified extrinsic mushroom body cells , 2004, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[6]  R. Menzel,et al.  A new ascending sensory tract to the calyces of the honeybee mushroom body, the subesophageal‐calycal tract , 2003, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[7]  M. Heisenberg Mushroom body memoir: from maps to models , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[8]  S. Morato,et al.  The role of vision and proprioception in the aversion of rats to the open arms of an elevated plus-maze , 2002, Behavioural Processes.

[9]  Jean-René Martin,et al.  Neuroendocrine control of a sexually dimorphic behavior by a few neurons of the pars intercerebralis in Drosophila , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[10]  W. Gronenberg,et al.  Segregation of visual input to the mushroom bodies in the honeybee (Apis mellifera) , 2002, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[11]  T. Kitamoto Conditional disruption of synaptic transmission induces male–male courtship behavior in Drosophila , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[12]  Glenn C. Turner,et al.  Oscillations and Sparsening of Odor Representations in the Mushroom Body , 2002, Science.

[13]  T. Préat,et al.  Localization of Long-Term Memory Within the Drosophila Mushroom Body , 2001, Science.

[14]  S. Morato,et al.  Vibrissal sense is not the main sensory modality in rat exploratory behavior in the elevated plus-maze , 2001, Behavioural Brain Research.

[15]  Ronald L. Davis,et al.  Molecular biology and anatomy of Drosophila olfactory associative learning , 2001, BioEssays : news and reviews in molecular, cellular and developmental biology.

[16]  R. Davis,et al.  The Role of Drosophila Mushroom Body Signaling in Olfactory Memory , 2001, Science.

[17]  T. Kitamoto Conditional modification of behavior in Drosophila by targeted expression of a temperature-sensitive shibire allele in defined neurons. , 2001, Journal of neurobiology.

[18]  Jean-René Martin,et al.  The Power Law Distribution for Walking-Time Intervals Correlates with the Ellipsoid-Body in Drosophila , 2001, Journal of neurogenetics.

[19]  W. Quinn,et al.  The amnesiac Gene Product Is Expressed in Two Neurons in the Drosophila Brain that Are Critical for Memory , 2000, Cell.

[20]  Jean-René Martin,et al.  Genetic identification of neurons controlling a sexually dimorphic behaviour , 2000, Current Biology.

[21]  M Heisenberg,et al.  Localization of a short-term memory in Drosophila. , 2000, Science.

[22]  T. Préat,et al.  Central brain postembryonic development in Drosophila: implication of genes expressed at the interhemispheric junction. , 2000, Journal of neurobiology.

[23]  K. Siwicki,et al.  Mushroom Body Ablation Impairs Short-Term Memory and Long-Term Memory of Courtship Conditioning in Drosophila melanogaster , 1999, Neuron.

[24]  J. Armstrong,et al.  Genetic analysis of the Drosophila ellipsoid body neuropil: organization and development of the central complex. , 1999, Journal of neurobiology.

[25]  L. Luo,et al.  Development of the Drosophila mushroom bodies: sequential generation of three distinct types of neurons from a neuroblast. , 1999, Development.

[26]  M. Heisenberg,et al.  Central complex substructures are required for the maintenance of locomotor activity in Drosophila melanogaster , 1999, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[27]  Li Liu,et al.  Context generalization in Drosophila visual learning requires the mushroom bodies , 1999, Nature.

[28]  B. Tabashnik,et al.  Development time and resistance to Bt crops , 1999, Nature.

[29]  N. Strausfeld,et al.  Mushroom bodies of the cockroach: Their participation in place memory , 1998, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[30]  N. Strausfeld,et al.  Mushroom bodies of the cockroach: Activity and identities of neurons recorded in freely moving animals , 1998, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[31]  M Heisenberg,et al.  Mushroom bodies suppress locomotor activity in Drosophila melanogaster. , 1998, Learning & memory.

[32]  R. Davis,et al.  Tripartite mushroom body architecture revealed by antigenic markers. , 1998, Learning & memory.

[33]  N. Strausfeld,et al.  The organization of extrinsic neurons and their implications in the functional roles of the mushroom bodies in Drosophila melanogaster Meigen. , 1998, Learning & memory.

[34]  J. Armstrong,et al.  Metamorphosis of the mushroom bodies; large-scale rearrangements of the neural substrates for associative learning and memory in Drosophila. , 1998, Learning & memory.

[35]  M. Heisenberg What do the mushroom bodies do for the insect brain? an introduction. , 1998, Learning & memory.

[36]  A. Ramos,et al.  Stress and emotionality: a multidimensional and genetic approach , 1997, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[37]  N. Strausfeld,et al.  Morphology and sensory modality of mushroom body extrinsic neurons in the brain of the cockroach, Periplaneta americana , 1997, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[38]  M. Freeman,et al.  Reiterative Use of the EGF Receptor Triggers Differentiation of All Cell Types in the Drosophila Eye , 1996, Cell.

[39]  M Heisenberg,et al.  Associative odor learning in Drosophila abolished by chemical ablation of mushroom bodies. , 1994, Science.

[40]  G. Vaysse,et al.  Immunocytochemical and learning studies of a Drosophila melanogaster neurological mutant, no-bridgeKS49 as an approach to the possible role of the central complex. , 1993, Journal of neurogenetics.

[41]  N. Perrimon,et al.  Targeted gene expression as a means of altering cell fates and generating dominant phenotypes. , 1993, Development.

[42]  R. Strauss,et al.  A higher control center of locomotor behavior in the Drosophila brain , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[43]  M Heisenberg,et al.  No-bridge of Drosophila melanogaster: portrait of a structural brain mutant of the central complex. , 1992, Journal of neurogenetics.

[44]  S. Morato,et al.  Effects of floor surface and environmental illumination on exploratory activity in the elevated plus-maze. , 1989, Brazilian journal of medical and biological research = Revista brasileira de pesquisas medicas e biologicas.

[45]  D. Treit,et al.  Thigmotaxis as a test for anxiolytic activity in rats , 1988, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[46]  K. Götz,et al.  Centrophobism in Drosophila melanogaster. I. Behavioral modification induced by ether , 1985 .

[47]  Karl Georg Götz,et al.  Centrophobism in Drosophila melanogaster. II. Physiological approach to search and search control , 1985 .

[48]  R. Morris,et al.  Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions , 1982, Nature.

[49]  R. Menzel,et al.  Localization of short‐term memory in the brain of the bee, Apis mellifera , 1980 .

[50]  J. Archer Tests for emotionality in rats and mice: a review. , 1973, Animal behaviour.