Slower Visuomotor Corrections with Unchanged Latency are Consistent with Optimal Adaptation to Increased Endogenous Noise in the Elderly

We analyzed age-related changes in motor response in a visuomotor compensatory tracking task. Subjects used a manipulandum to attempt to keep a displayed cursor at the center of a screen despite random perturbations to its location. Cross-correlation analysis of the perturbation and the subject response showed no age-related increase in latency until the onset of response to the perturbation, but substantial slowing of the response itself. Results are consistent with age-related deterioration in the ratio of signal to noise in visuomotor response. The task is such that it is tractable to use Bayesian and quadratic optimality assumptions to construct a model for behavior. This model assumes that behavior resembles an optimal controller subject to noise, and parametrizes response in terms of latency, willingness to expend effort, noise intensity, and noise bandwidth. The model is consistent with the data for all young (n = 12, age 20–30) and most elderly (n = 12, age 65–92) subjects. The model reproduces the latency result from the cross-correlation method. When presented with increased noise, the computational model reproduces the experimentally observed age-related slowing and the observed lack of increased latency. The model provides a precise way to quantitatively formulate the long-standing hypothesis that age-related slowing is an adaptation to increased noise.

[1]  Alan A. Hartley,et al.  Changing Role of the Speed of Processing Construct in the Cognitive Psychology of Human Aging , 2006 .

[2]  David L. Kleinman,et al.  A control theoretic approach to manned-vehicle systems analysis , 1971 .

[3]  竹安 数博,et al.  Time series analysis and its applications , 2007 .

[4]  R. Verleger,et al.  Aging and the Simon task. , 2002, Psychophysiology.

[5]  Karl M. Newell,et al.  Aging, Complexity, and Motor Performance , 2006 .

[6]  Zoubin Ghahramani,et al.  Computational principles of movement neuroscience , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[7]  P. Wason Ageing and Human Skill , 1960 .

[8]  V. Barnett,et al.  Applied Linear Statistical Models , 1975 .

[9]  D. Kleinman,et al.  Optimal stationary control of linear systems with control-dependent noise , 1969 .

[10]  F. Carp Handbook of the psychology of aging. , 1977 .

[11]  David L. Kleinman,et al.  The Human as an Optimal Controller and Information Processor , 1969 .

[12]  Karl M Newell,et al.  Aging and the time and frequency structure of force output variability. , 2003, Journal of applied physiology.

[13]  M G Paulin,et al.  Evolution of the cerebellum as a neuronal machine for Bayesian state estimation , 2005, Journal of neural engineering.

[14]  J. He,et al.  Feedback gains for correcting small perturbations to standing posture , 1989, Proceedings of the 28th IEEE Conference on Decision and Control,.

[15]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  Sensorimotor slowing with ageing is mediated by a functional dysregulation of motor-generation processes: evidence from high-resolution event-related potentials. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[16]  Raffaello D'Andrea,et al.  Visuomotor Optimality and its Utility in Parametrization of Response , 2008, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[17]  A. T. Welford,et al.  Relationships between reaction time and fatigue, stress, age and sex , 1980 .

[18]  K. J. Cole,et al.  Mechanisms for Age-Related Changes of Fingertip Forces during Precision Gripping and Lifting in Adults , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  B.D.O. Anderson,et al.  Controller design: moving from theory to practice , 1993, IEEE Control Systems.

[20]  J. Diedrichsen Optimal Task-Dependent Changes of Bimanual Feedback Control and Adaptation , 2007, Current Biology.

[21]  S. Griew,et al.  Ageing and Human Skill , 1958 .

[22]  Roger M Enoka,et al.  Different neural adjustments improve endpoint accuracy with practice in young and old adults. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  M. Falkenstein,et al.  Effects of aging on slowing of motor-response generation. , 2006, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[24]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[25]  A.D. Kuo,et al.  An optimal control model for analyzing human postural balance , 1995, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[26]  Alexandre Pouget,et al.  Optimal Sensorimotor Integration in Recurrent Cortical Networks: A Neural Implementation of Kalman Filters , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  Francisco J Valero-Cuevas,et al.  Manipulating the edge of instability. , 2007, Journal of biomechanics.

[28]  Konrad Paul Körding,et al.  The loss function of sensorimotor learning. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[29]  D. B. Lockhart,et al.  Optimal sensorimotor transformations for balance , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[30]  T. Salthouse,et al.  Adult age and the speed-accuracy trade-off. , 1979, Ergonomics.

[31]  H. Zelaznik,et al.  Motor-output variability: a theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts. , 1979, Psychological review.

[32]  G E Stelmach,et al.  Coactivation to reduce variability in the elderly. , 1998, Motor control.

[33]  J. Birren,et al.  Aging and speed of behavior: possible consequences for psychological functioning. , 1995, Annual review of psychology.

[34]  Karl M Newell,et al.  Age-related changes in complexity depend on task dynamics. , 2004, Journal of applied physiology.

[35]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Why Don't We Move Faster? Parkinson's Disease, Movement Vigor, and Implicit Motivation , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  Duane T. McRuer,et al.  Human dynamics in man-machine systems , 1980, Autom..

[37]  Denise C. Park,et al.  Handbook of the Psychology of Aging , 1979 .

[38]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Evidence for an error deadzone in compensatory tracking. , 1992, Journal of motor behavior.

[39]  B. Troost,et al.  The effects of age on normal saccadic characteristics and their variability , 1983, Vision Research.

[40]  B. Moor,et al.  Subspace state space system identification for industrial processes , 1998 .

[41]  R W Baloh,et al.  Age-related changes in visual tracking. , 1994, Journal of gerontology.

[42]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Making smooth moves , 2022 .

[43]  D. Knill,et al.  The Bayesian brain: the role of uncertainty in neural coding and computation , 2004, Trends in Neurosciences.

[44]  Emanuel Todorov,et al.  Stochastic Optimal Control and Estimation Methods Adapted to the Noise Characteristics of the Sensorimotor System , 2005, Neural Computation.

[45]  E. Todorov Optimality principles in sensorimotor control , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[46]  T. Salthouse Aging and measures of processing speed , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[47]  W. Shebilske,et al.  Motor Learning and Control , 1993 .

[48]  A. T. Welford,et al.  The fundamentals of skill , 1968 .

[49]  Kelly J. Cole,et al.  Old age affects fingertip forces when restraining an unpredictably loaded object , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[50]  Michael H. Kutner Applied Linear Statistical Models , 1974 .

[51]  R J Jagacinski,et al.  Quantifying the performance limitations of older and younger adults in a target acquisition task. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[52]  J. Pratt,et al.  Aging and movement: variability of force pulses for saccadic eye movements. , 1998, Psychology and aging.

[53]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.10 No.7 July 2006 Special Issue: Probabilistic models of cognition Bayesian decision theory in sensorimotor control , 2022 .