People in the Paleolithic could access the whole spectrum of consciousness: response to Helvenston

Three kinds of hallucinations have repeatedly been identified in the literature on altered states of consciousness (ASCs): visions of (1) geometric forms, (2) figures and objects, and (3) complete scenes. Lewis-Williams’ neuropsychological model draws on these reports to gain insights into the minds of Paleolithic people, on the basis of shared neurobiology and given comparative ethnographic data on ritualized ASCs. Helvenston has long rejected this model because in many ASCs hallucinations do not always adhere to a strict 1-2-3 sequence, because they do not always feature animals, and because people do not always lose their critical faculties. She is right, but she is attacking a straw man because these criteria are her own. Helvenston’s claims about the effects of psychoactive compounds and sensory deprivation are also questionable. It remains an open question how our Turing pattern model relates to more figurative forms of hallucinations.

[1]  Tom Froese,et al.  Turing instabilities in biology, culture, and consciousness? On the enactive origins of symbolic material culture , 2013, Adapt. Behav..

[2]  J. Borovička,et al.  The wood-rotting bluing Psilocybe species in Central Europe - an identification key , 2008 .

[3]  R J Sullivan,et al.  Psychotropic substance-seeking: evolutionary pathology or adaptation? , 2002, Addiction.

[4]  Tom Froese,et al.  Are altered states of consciousness detrimental, neutral or helpful for the origins of symbolic cognition? A response to Hodgson and Lewis-Williams , 2014, Adapt. Behav..

[5]  Alan Radley Image and imagination , 2012 .

[6]  Patricia Helvenston,et al.  Comments on the paper “Are altered states of consciousness detrimental, useful or helpful for the origins of symbolic cognition? A response to Hodgson and Lewis Williams”, by T Froese, A Woodward and T Ikegami , 2014, Adapt. Behav..

[7]  S. Deshmukh,et al.  Fungi from high nitrogen environments-ammonia fungi: eco-physiological aspects. , 2009 .

[8]  L. Malafouris Before and Beyond Representation: Towards an enactive conception of the Palaeolithic image , 2007 .

[9]  R. H. Scott,et al.  Effects of decreased variation in the sensory environment. , 1954, Canadian journal of psychology.

[10]  Lotfi B Merabet,et al.  Visual Hallucinations During Prolonged Blindfolding in Sighted Subjects , 2004, Journal of neuro-ophthalmology : the official journal of the North American Neuro-Ophthalmology Society.

[11]  H. Lüders,et al.  Comments , 2002, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[12]  Joan M. Vastokas,et al.  The Signs of All Times: Entoptic Phenomena in Upper Palaeolithic Art [and Comments and Reply] , 1988, Current Anthropology.

[13]  J. Zubek,et al.  Perceptual changes after prolonged sensory isolation (darkness and silence). , 1961, Canadian journal of psychology.

[14]  J. K. Misra,et al.  Fungi from different environments , 2009 .

[15]  J. Lewis-Williams,et al.  The Mind in the Cave , 2002 .

[16]  S. Deshmukh,et al.  The Hallucinogenic Mushrooms: Diversity, Traditions, Use and Abuse with Special Reference to the Genus Psilocybe , 2019, Fungi from Different Environments.

[17]  José-Luis Díaz,et al.  Sacred plants and visionary consciousness 5 , 2010 .