Learning about pain: the neural substrate of the prediction error for aversive events.

Associative learning is thought to depend on detecting mismatches between actual and expected experiences. With functional magnetic resonance imaging (FMRI), we studied brain activity during different types of mismatch in a paradigm where contrasting-colored lights signaled the delivery of painful heat, nonpainful warmth, or no stimulation. When painful heat stimulation was unexpected, there was increased FMRI signal intensity in areas of the hippocampus, superior frontal gyrus, cerebellum, and superior parietal gyrus that was not found with mismatch between expectation and delivery of nonpainful warmth stimulation. When painful heat stimulation was unexpectedly omitted, the FMRI signal intensity decreased in the left superior parietal gyrus and increased in the other regions. These contrasting activation patterns correspond to two different mismatch concepts in theories of associative learning (Rescorla-Wagner, temporal difference vs. Pearce-Hall, Mackintosh). Searching for interventions to specifically modulate activation of these brain regions therefore offers an approach to identifying new treatments for chronic pain, which often has a substantial associative learning component.

[1]  O. Mowrer Preparatory set (expectancy)—a determinant in motivation and learning. , 1938 .

[2]  E. N. Sokolov Higher nervous functions; the orienting reflex. , 1963, Annual review of physiology.

[3]  B. Campbell,et al.  Punishment and aversive behavior , 1969 .

[4]  W. F. Prokasy,et al.  Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory. , 1972 .

[5]  N. Mackintosh A Theory of Attention: Variations in the Associability of Stimuli with Reinforcement , 1975 .

[6]  M. Singleton The Human Nervous System, ed 2 , 1976 .

[7]  A. Black Functions of the septo-hippocampal system , 1978, Nature.

[8]  J D Troup,et al.  Outline of a Fear-Avoidance Model of exaggerated pain perception--I. , 1983, Behaviour research and therapy.

[9]  A. Galaburda,et al.  Inferior parietal lobule. Divergent architectonic asymmetries in the human brain. , 1984, Archives of neurology.

[10]  J. Gray The neuropsychology of anxiety. , 1985, Issues in mental health nursing.

[11]  M. Weisenberg Psychological intervention for the control of pain. , 1987, Behaviour research and therapy.

[12]  J. Baron Thinking and Deciding , 2023 .

[13]  W. J. Nowack Neurobiology of Neocortex , 1989, Neurology.

[14]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Posterior parietal cortex in rhesus monkey: I. Parcellation of areas based on distinctive limbic and sensory corticocortical connections , 1989, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[15]  R. Rescorla Evidence for an association between the discriminative stimulus and the response-outcome association in instrumental learning. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[16]  J. Steinmetz,et al.  Dorsal accessory inferior olive activity diminishes during acquisition of the rabbit classically conditioned eyelid response , 1991, Brain Research.

[17]  D. Siddle,et al.  Orienting, habituation, and resource allocation: an associative analysis. , 1991, Psychophysiology.

[18]  E. Cabanis,et al.  The Human Brain: Surface, Three-Dimensional Sectional Anatomy and Mri , 1991 .

[19]  P. Luiten,et al.  Amygdala kindling‐induced seizures selectively impair spatial memory. 2. Effects on hippocampal neuronal and glial muscarinic acetylcholine receptor , 1992, Hippocampus.

[20]  R. Passingham The frontal lobes and voluntary action , 1993 .

[21]  D. Pandya,et al.  Prelunate, occipitotemporal, and parahippocampal projections to the basis pontis in rhesus monkey , 1993, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[22]  E. Tulving,et al.  Novelty encoding networks in the human brain: positron emission tomography data. , 1994, Neuroreport.

[23]  S. Wise,et al.  Neuronal activity in the supplementary eye field during acquisition of conditional oculomotor associations. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  D J Krupa,et al.  Inactivation of the superior cerebellar peduncle blocks expression but not acquisition of the rabbit's classically conditioned eye-blink response. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[25]  R. Knight Contribution of human hippocampal region to novelty detection , 1996, Nature.

[26]  J. R. Baker,et al.  The hippocampal formation participates in novel picture encoding: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[27]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  Functional Mapping of Human Learning: A Positron Emission Tomography Activation Study of Eyeblink Conditioning , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Face encoding and recognition in the human brain. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[29]  C. Chapman Limbic processes and the affective dimension of pain. , 1996, Progress in brain research.

[30]  Charles L. Wilson,et al.  Single Neuron Activity in Human Hippocampus and Amygdala during Recognition of Faces and Objects , 1997, Neuron.

[31]  Pearce Animal learning and cognition , 1997 .

[32]  G H Glover,et al.  Separate neural bases of two fundamental memory processes in the human medial temporal lobe. , 1997, Science.

[33]  R. Passingham,et al.  The left parietal cortex and motor attention , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  P. Holland,et al.  Removal of Cholinergic Input to Rat Posterior Parietal Cortex Disrupts Incremental Processing of Conditioned Stimuli , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  E. Kang,et al.  Hippocampal modulation of cingulo‐thalamic neuronal activity and discriminative avoidance learning in rabbits , 1998, Hippocampus.

[36]  Joseph E LeDoux,et al.  Human Amygdala Activation during Conditioned Fear Acquisition and Extinction: a Mixed-Trial fMRI Study , 1998, Neuron.

[37]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Human cortical mechanisms of visual attention during orienting and search. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[38]  R. C. Honey,et al.  Hippocampal Lesions Disrupt an Associative Mismatch Process , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  P. Redgrave,et al.  Is the short-latency dopamine response too short to signal reward error? , 1999, Trends in Neurosciences.

[40]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Segregating the functions of human hippocampus. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[41]  Christoph Braun,et al.  Coherence of gamma-band EEG activity as a basis for associative learning , 1999, Nature.

[42]  Joel R. Meyer,et al.  A large-scale distributed network for covert spatial attention: further anatomical delineation based on stringent behavioural and cognitive controls. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[43]  B. Krauss,et al.  Differentiating cortical areas related to pain perception from stimulus identification: temporal analysis of fMRI activity. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  K L Casey,et al.  Forebrain mechanisms of nociception and pain: analysis through imaging. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[45]  M. Bushnell,et al.  Pain perception: is there a role for primary somatosensory cortex? , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[46]  J. Maisog,et al.  Pain intensity processing within the human brain: a bilateral, distributed mechanism. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[47]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Dissociating pain from its anticipation in the human brain. , 1999, Science.

[48]  B. Kopp,et al.  Brain mechanisms of selective learning: event-related potentials provide evidence for error-driven learning in humans , 2000, Biological Psychology.