Is Human-Robot Interaction More Competitive Between Groups Than Between Individuals?

As robots, both individually and in groups, become more prevalent in everyday contexts (e.g., schools, workplaces, educational and caregiving institutions), it is possible that they will be perceived as outgroups, or come into competition for resources with humans. Research indicates that some of the psychological effects of intergroup interaction common in humans translate to human-robot interaction (HRI). In this paper, we examine how intergroup competition, like that among humans, translates to HRI. Specifically, we examined how Number of Humans (1, 3) and Number of Robots (1, 3) affect behavioral competition on dilemma tasks and survey ratings of perceived threat, emotion, and motivation (fear, greed, and outperformance). We also examined the effect of perceived group entitativity (i.e., cohesiveness) on competition motivation. Like in social psychological literature, these results indicate that groups of humans (especially entitative groups) showed more greed-based motivation and competition toward robots than individual humans did. However, we did not find evidence that number of robots had an effect on fear-based motivation or competition against them unless the robot groups were perceived as highly entitative. Our data also show the intriguing finding that participants displayed more fear of and competed slightly more against robots that matched their number. Future research should more deeply examine this novel pattern of results compared to one-on-one HRI and typical group dynamics in social psychology.

[1]  John Thibaut,et al.  On the measurement of social orientations in the minimal group paradigm , 1983 .

[2]  Rupert Brown,et al.  Superordinate goals and intergroup behaviour: The effect of role ambiguity and status on intergroup attitudes and task performance , 1987 .

[3]  Hein F. M. Lodewijkx,et al.  INDIVIDUAL-GROUP CONTINUITY IN COOPERATION AND COMPETITION UNDER VARYING COMMUNICATION CONDITIONS , 2015 .

[4]  Brian P. Meier,et al.  A comparison of human aggression committed by groups and individuals: An interindividual–intergroup discontinuity☆ , 2004 .

[5]  Nir Halevy,et al.  Conflict Templates , 2013 .

[6]  M. Banaji,et al.  Group entitativity and group perception: associations between physical features and psychological judgment. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[7]  Takayuki Kanda,et al.  Escaping from Children’s Abuse of Social Robots , 2015, 2015 10th ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI).

[8]  Chester A. Insko,et al.  Interindividual‐intergroup discontinuity as a joint function of acting as a group and interacting with a group , 2007 .

[9]  David F. Gruber Three's company , 2009, Nature Medicine.

[10]  Kenneth A. Graetz,et al.  Individual-Group Discontinuity: Further Evidence for Mediation by Fear and Greed , 1993 .

[11]  C. Insko,et al.  Memory for and experience of differential competitive behavior of individuals and groups. , 1996, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[12]  Rick H. Hoyle,et al.  Individual-Group Discontinuity as a Function of Fear and Greed , 1990 .

[13]  D. Wegner,et al.  What do I think you're doing? Action identification and mind attribution. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[14]  P. Dayan,et al.  Differential Encoding of Losses and Gains in the Human Striatum , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  Wan Ling Chang,et al.  The effect of group size on people's attitudes and cooperative behaviors toward robots in interactive gameplay , 2012, 2012 IEEE RO-MAN: The 21st IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication.

[16]  L. Doob The psychology of social norms. , 1937 .

[17]  Selma Sabanovic,et al.  Teammates first: Favoring ingroup robots over outgroup humans , 2017, 2017 26th IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication (RO-MAN).

[18]  Steven L. Neuberg,et al.  INTERPERSONAL RELATIONS AND GROUP PROCESSES Different Emotional Reactions to Different Groups : A Sociofunctional Threat-Based Approach to “ Prejudice ” , 2005 .

[19]  Bharat Maldé,et al.  Interpersonal relations — a theory of interdependence , 1979 .

[20]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Anomalies: The Endowment Effect, Loss Aversion, and Status Quo Bias , 1991 .

[21]  Eliot R. Smith,et al.  Intergroup emotions and intergroup relations. , 2008 .

[22]  F. H. Hankins,et al.  The Psychology of Social Norms , 1937 .

[23]  D. Campbell Common fate, similarity, and other indices of the status of aggregates of persons as social entities , 2007 .

[24]  Jack L. Vevea,et al.  Beyond the group mind: a quantitative review of the interindividual-intergroup discontinuity effect. , 2003, Psychological bulletin.

[25]  Eliot R. Smith,et al.  Intergroup emotions: explaining offensive action tendencies in an intergroup context. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[26]  W. Stephan Intergroup Anxiety , 2014, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[27]  Selma Sabanovic,et al.  Negative Attitudes toward minimalistic Robots with intragroup communication styles , 2014, The 23rd IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication.

[28]  H. Kelley,et al.  Interpersonal relations: A theory of interdependence , 1978 .

[29]  Christoph Bartneck,et al.  Can we control it? Autonomous robots threaten human identity, uniqueness, safety, and resources , 2017, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[30]  Selma Sabanovic,et al.  Three's company, or a crowd?: The effects of robot number and behavior on HRI in Japan and the USA , 2015, Robotics: Science and Systems.

[31]  Frédéric Kaplan,et al.  Who is Afraid of the Humanoid? Investigating Cultural Differences in the Acceptance of Robots , 2004, Int. J. Humanoid Robotics.

[32]  C. Insko,et al.  Interindividual-Intergroup Discontinuity in the Prisoner's Dilemma Game: How Common Fate, Proximity, and Similarity Affect Intergroup Competition , 2012 .

[33]  Oliver Kirchkamp,et al.  Social categorization and group-motivated interindividual–intergroup discontinuity , 2013 .

[34]  Selma Sabanovic,et al.  Rabble of Robots Effects: Number and Type of Robots Modulates Attitudes, Emotions, and Stereotypes , 2015, 2015 10th ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI).

[35]  Selma Sabanovic,et al.  Threatening Flocks and Mindful Snowflakes: How Group Entitativity Affects Perceptions of Robots , 2017, 2017 12th ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI.

[36]  Lowell Gaertner,et al.  Intragroup social influence and intergroup competition. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[37]  Rick H. Hoyle,et al.  Individual-group discontinuity: The role of a consensus rule , 1988 .

[38]  Kenneth J. Gergen,et al.  Deviance In Dark , 1973 .

[39]  T. Postmes,et al.  Intergroup distinctiveness and differentiation: a meta-analytic integration. , 2004, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[40]  Wan Ling Chang,et al.  Interaction Expands Function: Social Shaping of the Therapeutic Robot PARO in a Nursing Home , 2015, 2015 10th ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI).

[41]  Steven J. Stroessner,et al.  The Robotic Social Attributes Scale (RoSAS): Development and Validation , 2017, 2017 12th ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI.

[42]  Miles Hewstone,et al.  Prejudice, stereotyping and discrimination: Theoretical and empirical overview , 2010 .

[43]  Chester A. Insko,et al.  Differential distrust of groups and individuals. , 1998 .

[44]  Chester A. Insko,et al.  Explanations of interindividual – intergroup discontinuity: A review of the evidence , 2007 .