Behavioral and event-related potential distraction effects with regularly occurring auditory deviants.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] I. Winkler,et al. Involuntary Attention and Distractibility as Evaluated with Event-Related Brain Potentials , 2000, Audiology and Neurotology.
[2] Erich Schröger,et al. Response repetition vs. response change modulates behavioral and electrophysiological effects of distraction. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[3] E. Schröger,et al. A comparison of auditory and visual distraction effects: behavioral and event-related indices. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[4] C. Escera,et al. Electrical responses reveal the temporal dynamics of brain events during involuntary attention switching , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.
[5] D. Friedman,et al. The novelty P3: an event-related brain potential (ERP) sign of the brain's evaluation of novelty , 2001, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[6] W. Ritter,et al. Storage of information in transient auditory memory. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[7] E. Schröger,et al. Attentional orienting and reorienting is indicated by human event‐related brain potentials , 1998, Neuroreport.
[8] F. Perrin,et al. Separate Representation of Stimulus Frequency, Intensity, and Duration in Auditory Sensory Memory: An Event-Related Potential and Dipole-Model Analysis , 1995, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[9] D Deacon,et al. Automatic change detection: does the auditory system use representations of individual stimulus features or gestalts? , 1998, Psychophysiology.
[10] D. Vernon,et al. Event-Related Brain Potential Correlates of Human Auditory Sensory Memory-Trace Formation , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[11] E. Schröger,et al. Behavioral and electrophysiological effects of task-irrelevant sound change: a new distraction paradigm. , 1998, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[12] Erich Schröger,et al. Working memory controls involuntary attention switching: evidence from an auditory distraction paradigm , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.
[13] Stefan Berti,et al. Examining task-dependencies of different attentional processes as reflected in the P3a and reorienting negativity components of the human event-related brain potential , 2006, Neuroscience Letters.
[14] E. Schröger,et al. Bottom-up influences on working memory: behavioral and electrophysiological distraction varies with distractor strength. , 2004, Experimental psychology.
[15] E. Schröger,et al. Auditory distraction with different presentation rates: an event-related potential and behavioral study , 2003, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[16] Carles Escera,et al. Attention capture by auditory significant stimuli: semantic analysis follows attention switching , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.
[17] Erich Schröger,et al. Auditory distraction: event-related potential and behavioral indices , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[18] J. S. Nairne. A feature model of immediate memory , 1990, Memory & cognition.
[19] R. Näätänen,et al. The mismatch negativity (MMN): towards the optimal paradigm , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[20] Erich Schröger,et al. Distraction effects in vision: behavioral and event-related potential indices , 2004, Neuroreport.
[21] E. Schröger. On the detection of auditory deviations: a pre-attentive activation model. , 1997, Psychophysiology.
[22] H. Zimmer,et al. Habituation of the orienting response as reflected by the skin conductance response and by endogenous event-related brain potentials. , 2006, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[23] I. Winkler,et al. Top-down control over involuntary attention switching in the auditory modality , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[24] Erich Schröger,et al. Visual distraction: a behavioral and event-related brain potential study in humans , 2006, Neuroreport.