Integration of proprioceptive and visual position-information: An experimentally supported model.

To localize one's hand, i.e., to find out its position with respect to the body, humans may use proprioceptive information or visual information or both. It is still not known how the CNS combines simultaneous proprioceptive and visual information. In this study, we investigate in what position in a horizontal plane a hand is localized on the basis of simultaneous proprioceptive and visual information and compare this to the positions in which it is localized on the basis of proprioception only and vision only. Seated at a table, subjects matched target positions on the table top with their unseen left hand under the table. The experiment consisted of three series. In each of these series, the target positions were presented in three conditions: by vision only, by proprioception only, or by both vision and proprioception. In one of the three series, the visual information was veridical. In the other two, it was modified by prisms that displaced the visual field to the left and to the right, respectively. The results show that the mean of the positions indicated in the condition with both vision and proprioception generally lies off the straight line through the means of the other two conditions. In most cases the mean lies on the side predicted by a model describing the integration of multisensory information. According to this model, the visual information and the proprioceptive information are weighted with direction-dependent weights, the weights being related to the direction-dependent precision of the information in such a way that the available information is used very efficiently. Because the proposed model also can explain the unexpectedly small sizes of the variable errors in the localization of a seen hand that were reported earlier, there is strong evidence to support this model. The results imply that the CNS has knowledge about the direction-dependent precision of the proprioceptive and visual information.

[1]  Victor Horsley,et al.  UPON THE ORIENTATION OF POINTS IN SPACE BY THE MUSCULAR, ARTHRODIAL, AND TACTILE SENSES OF THE UPPER LIMBS IN NORMAL INDIVIDUALS AND IN BLIND PERSONS , 1906 .

[2]  J. I The Design of Experiments , 1936, Nature.

[3]  B. J. Winer Statistical Principles in Experimental Design , 1992 .

[4]  Hermann von Helmholtz,et al.  Treatise on Physiological Optics , 1962 .

[5]  C R HAMILTON,et al.  INTERMANUAL TRANSFER OF ADAPTATION TO PRISMS. , 1964, The American journal of psychology.

[6]  H. Pick,et al.  Visual capture produced by prism spectacles , 1965 .

[7]  M M Cohen,et al.  Continuous versus Terminal Visual Feedback in Prism Aftereffects , 1967, Perceptual and motor skills.

[8]  D. H. Warren,et al.  Sensory conflict in judgments of spatial direction , 1969 .

[9]  D. H. Warren,et al.  Intermodality relations in localization in blind and sighted people , 1970 .

[10]  L. K. Canon Intermodality inconsistency of input and directed attention as determinants of the nature of adaptation. , 1970, Journal of experimental psychology.

[11]  L. K. Canon,et al.  Influence of concurrent and terminal exposure conditions on the nature of perceptual adaptation. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.

[12]  L. K. Canon,et al.  Directed attention and maladaptive "adaptation" to displacement of the visual field. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.

[13]  D. H. Warren,et al.  Visual-proprioceptive interaction under large amounts of conflict. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.

[14]  John M. Foley,et al.  Visually directed pointing as a function of target distance, direction, and available cues , 1972 .

[15]  J A Kelso,et al.  Allocation of attention and the locus of adaptation to displaced vision. , 1975, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  D H Warren,et al.  On the plasticity of visual-proprioceptive bias effects. , 1978, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  James R. Lackner,et al.  Visual direction depends on the operation of spatial constancy mechanisms: the oculobrachial illusion , 1978, Neuroscience Letters.

[18]  R B Welch,et al.  An examination of the relationship between visual capture and prism adaptation , 1979, Perception & psychophysics.

[19]  David H. Warren,et al.  Response factors in intermodality localization under conflict conditions , 1980 .

[20]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Neuronal population coding of movement direction. , 1986, Science.

[21]  A. Crowe,et al.  Proprioceptive Accuracy in Two Dimensions , 1987, Perceptual and motor skills.

[22]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  New directions for oculomotor research , 1990, Vision Research.

[23]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Cerebral cortical mechanisms of reaching movements. , 1992, Science.

[24]  J. Lackner,et al.  The role of brachial muscle spindle signals in assignment of visual direction. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  Charles H. Anderson,et al.  BASIC ELEMENTS OF BIOLOGICAL COMPUTATIONAL SYSTEMS , 1994 .

[26]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Vectorial coding of movement: vision, proprioception, or both? , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  J. Soechting,et al.  Somatosensory cortical activity in relation to arm posture: nonuniform spatial tuning. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  T. Sanger,et al.  Probability density estimation for the interpretation of neural population codes. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  G. M. Redding,et al.  Adaptive spatial alignment and strategic perceptual-motor control. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Computational models of sensorimotor integration , 1997 .

[31]  Pietro G. Morasso,et al.  Self-Organization, Computational Maps, and Motor Control , 1997 .

[32]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning , 1998, Nature.

[33]  Alexandre Pouget,et al.  Probabilistic Interpretation of Population Codes , 1996, Neural Computation.

[34]  Anne C. Sittig,et al.  The precision of proprioceptive position sense , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.