Age-related differences in reallocating cognitive resources when dealing with interruptions
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] H Eyrolle,et al. The effects of interruptions in work activity: field and laboratory results. , 2000, Applied ergonomics.
[2] M. Molnár,et al. Age-related changes of frontal-midline theta is predictive of efficient memory maintenance , 2014, Neuroscience.
[3] R. Nigbur,et al. Theta power as a marker for cognitive interference , 2011, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[4] M. Frank,et al. Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[5] R. Chan,et al. Effects of aging, planning, and interruption on complex prospective memory , 2013, Neuropsychological rehabilitation.
[6] T. Salthouse. The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. , 1996, Psychological review.
[7] S. Reimers,et al. Task switching across the life span: effects of age on general and specific switch costs. , 2005, Developmental psychology.
[8] P. Verhaeghen. Aging and Executive Control: Reports of a Demise Greatly Exaggerated , 2011, Current directions in psychological science.
[9] M. D’Esposito. Working memory. , 2008, Handbook of clinical neurology.
[10] Jonathan D. Wallis,et al. Executive control processes underlying multi-item working memory , 2014, Nature Neuroscience.
[11] Michael X. Cohen,et al. Fronto-parietal network oscillations reveal relationship between working memory capacity and cognitive control , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[12] I. Deary,et al. Reaction Time, Age, and Cognitive Ability: Longitudinal Findings from Age 16 to 63 Years in Representative Population Samples , 2005 .
[13] A. Dale,et al. Age-related alterations in white matter microstructure measured by diffusion tensor imaging , 2005, Neurobiology of Aging.
[14] J. Cavanagh,et al. Frontal midline theta reflects anxiety and cognitive control: Meta-analytic evidence , 2015, Journal of Physiology-Paris.
[15] Stefan Everling,et al. A long-range fronto-parietal 5- to 10-Hz network predicts "top-down" controlled guidance in a task-switch paradigm. , 2014, Cerebral cortex.
[16] J. Yesavage,et al. Context processing in older adults: evidence for a theory relating cognitive control to neurobiology in healthy aging. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[17] Axel Mecklinger,et al. Age differences in attentional control: an event-related potential approach. , 2005, Psychophysiology.
[18] W. Singer,et al. Dynamic predictions: Oscillations and synchrony in top–down processing , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[19] Arnaud Delorme,et al. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.
[20] R. Ratcliff,et al. Aging and individual differences in rapid two-choice decisions , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[21] C. J. Lahar,et al. Age Differences in the Magnitude of the Attentional Blink , 2001 .
[22] John Duncan,et al. Shape-specific preparatory activity mediates attention to targets in human visual cortex , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[23] N. Cowan. The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity , 2001, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[24] F. Craik,et al. Divided attention in younger and older adults: effects of strategy and relatedness on memory performance and secondary task costs. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[25] T. Braver,et al. Explaining the many varieties of working memory variation: Dual mechanisms of cognitive control. , 2007 .
[26] A. Baddeley. Working memory: looking back and looking forward , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[27] R. Knight,et al. Oscillatory Dynamics of Prefrontal Cognitive Control , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[28] Deborah A. Boehm-Davis,et al. Do Interruptions Affect the Quality of Work? , 2013 .
[29] Faith M. Gunning-Dixon,et al. Aging of cerebral white matter: a review of MRI findings , 2009, International journal of geriatric psychiatry.
[30] Adam Gazzaley,et al. Predictive knowledge of stimulus relevance does not influence top-down suppression of irrelevant information in older adults , 2010, Cortex.
[31] M. Corbetta,et al. Frontoparietal Cortex Controls Spatial Attention through Modulation of Anticipatory Alpha Rhythms , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[32] M. Falkenstein,et al. Age-Related Effects on ERP and Oscillatory EEG-Dynamics in a 2-Back Task , 2014 .
[33] J. Cellier,et al. Interference between switched tasks , 1992 .
[34] Tao Liu,et al. Strategic Modulation of Cognitive Control , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[35] N. Volkow,et al. Association between age-related decline in brain dopamine activity and impairment in frontal and cingulate metabolism. , 2000, The American journal of psychiatry.
[36] Yoko Yamaguchi,et al. Human cortical circuits for central executive function emerge by theta phase synchronization , 2007, NeuroImage.
[37] T. Braver. The variable nature of cognitive control: a dual mechanisms framework , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[38] S. Resnick,et al. Longitudinal pattern of regional brain volume change differentiates normal aging from MCI , 2009, Neurology.
[39] Kim M Chronister. Divided attention. , 2003, Rehab management.
[40] M. J. Emerson,et al. The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.
[41] Víctor M. González,et al. "Constant, constant, multi-tasking craziness": managing multiple working spheres , 2004, CHI.
[42] Adam Gazzaley,et al. In Brief , 2011, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[43] C. Stam,et al. Phase lag index: Assessment of functional connectivity from multi channel EEG and MEG with diminished bias from common sources , 2007, Human brain mapping.
[44] Dylan M. Jones,et al. Interruption of the Tower of London task: support for a goal-activation approach. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[45] Michael X Cohen. A better way to define and describe Morlet wavelets for time-frequency analysis , 2018 .
[46] E. Yund,et al. Age-related slowing of response selection and production in a visual choice reaction time task , 2015, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[47] John J. B. Allen,et al. Theta lingua franca: a common mid-frontal substrate for action monitoring processes. , 2012, Psychophysiology.
[48] M. Botvinick. Conflict monitoring and decision making: Reconciling two perspectives on anterior cingulate function , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[49] J. Gregory Trafton,et al. Memory for goals: an activation-based model , 2002, Cogn. Sci..
[50] T. Salthouse. Aging and measures of processing speed , 2000, Biological Psychology.
[51] Theodore P. Zanto,et al. Causal role of the prefrontal cortex in top-down modulation of visual processing and working memory , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.
[52] S. Kastner,et al. A neural basis for real-world visual search in human occipitotemporal cortex , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[53] Gloria Mark,et al. The cost of interrupted work: more speed and stress , 2008, CHI.
[54] M. Molnár,et al. Frontal midline theta connectivity is related to efficiency of WM maintenance and is affected by aging , 2014, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.
[55] Andrew B. Leber,et al. Coordination of Voluntary and Stimulus-Driven Attentional Control in Human Cortex , 2005, Psychological science.
[56] U. Lindenberger,et al. Adult age differences in task switching. , 2000, Psychology and aging.
[57] P. Fries. Rhythms for Cognition: Communication through Coherence , 2015, Neuron.
[58] R. Cabeza,et al. Frontal Lobes and Aging , 2013 .
[59] R. West,et al. An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. , 1996, Psychological bulletin.
[60] Gerhard Rinkenauer,et al. The interconnection of mental fatigue and aging: An EEG study. , 2017, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[61] R. Davidson,et al. The integration of negative affect, pain and cognitive control in the cingulate cortex , 2011, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[62] T. Ishihara,et al. Multivariate analytic study of EEG and mental activity in juvenile delinquents. , 1972, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[63] Jan R. Wessel,et al. Frontal theta is a signature of successful working memory manipulation , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.
[64] Jiajie Zhang,et al. A Concept Analysis of the Phenomenon Interruption , 2007, ANS. Advances in nursing science.
[65] Vijeth Iyengar,et al. Less wiring, more firing: low-performing older adults compensate for impaired white matter with greater neural activity. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.
[66] F. Clark,et al. AGING AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCIES , 2011 .
[67] A. Gazzaley,et al. Distinct mechanisms for the impact of distraction and interruption on working memory in aging , 2012, Neurobiology of Aging.
[68] R Core Team,et al. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .
[69] I. Peretz,et al. Effect of Age on Attentional Control in Dual-Tasking , 2017, Experimental aging research.
[70] R. T. Pivik,et al. Guidelines for the recording and quantitative analysis of electroencephalographic activity in research contexts. , 1993, Psychophysiology.
[71] Daryl E. Wilson,et al. Control of Spatial and Feature-Based Attention in Frontoparietal Cortex , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[72] A. Engel,et al. Cognitive control during audiovisual working memory engages frontotemporal theta-band interactions , 2017, Scientific Reports.
[73] Christopher A. Monk,et al. Recovering From Interruptions: Implications for Driver Distraction Research , 2004, Hum. Factors.
[74] Lynn Hasher,et al. Inhibitory deficit theory: Recent developments in a "new view" , 2007 .
[75] P. Schwartzkroin,et al. Neural mechanisms. , 1994, Science.
[76] I. Deary,et al. Age and sex differences in reaction time in adulthood: results from the United Kingdom Health and Lifestyle Survey. , 2006, Psychology and aging.
[77] Adam Gazzaley,et al. Age-related deficits in component processes of working memory. , 2007, Neuropsychology.
[78] P. Verhaeghen,et al. Aging and verbal memory span: a meta-analysis. , 2005, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.
[79] Marius V Peelen,et al. Preparatory attention in visual cortex , 2017, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[80] G. Mangun,et al. The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[81] Dwight J. Kravitz,et al. Attentional control: Temporal relationships within the fronto-parietal network , 2012, Neuropsychologia.
[82] Ulman Lindenberger,et al. Adult age differences in task switching , 2000 .
[83] P. König,et al. Oscillatory brain activity during multisensory attention reflects activation, disinhibition, and cognitive control , 2016, Scientific Reports.
[84] Jessica Sänger,et al. When compensation fails: Attentional deficits in healthy ageing caused by visual distraction , 2012, Neuropsychologia.