Development of relational reasoning during adolescence.

Non-linear changes in behaviour and in brain activity during adolescent development have been reported in a variety of cognitive tasks. These developmental changes are often interpreted as being a consequence of changes in brain structure, including non-linear changes in grey matter volumes, which occur during adolescence. However, very few studies have attempted to combine behavioural, functional and structural data. This multi-method approach is the one we took in the current study, which was designed to investigate developmental changes in behaviour and brain activity during relational reasoning, the simultaneous integration of multiple relations. We used a relational reasoning task known to recruit rostrolateral prefrontal cortex (RLPFC), a region that undergoes substantial structural changes during adolescence. The task was administered to female participants in a behavioural (N = 178, 7-27 years) and an fMRI study (N = 37, 11-30 years). Non-linear changes in accuracy were observed, with poorer performance during mid-adolescence. fMRI and VBM results revealed a complex picture of linear and possibly non-linear changes with age. Performance and structural changes partly accounted for changes with age in RLPFC and medial superior frontal gyrus activity but not for a decrease in activation in the anterior insula/frontal operculum between mid-adolescence and adulthood. These functional changes might instead reflect the maturation of neurocognitive strategies.

[1]  K. Holyoak,et al.  A System for Relational Reasoning in Human Prefrontal Cortex , 1999 .

[2]  Iroise Dumontheil,et al.  Development of rostral prefrontal cortex and cognitive and behavioural disorders , 2008, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[3]  D. Lewis,et al.  Development of the Prefrontal Cortex during Adolescence: Insights into Vulnerable Neural Circuits in Schizophrenia , 1997, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[4]  Robert Young,et al.  Transition matters: pupils’ experiences of the primary–secondary school transition in the West of Scotland and consequences for well‐being and attainment , 2010 .

[5]  Iroise Dumontheil,et al.  The gateway hypothesis of rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) function , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[6]  Kuei Yuan Tseng,et al.  Dopamine modulation of prefrontal cortical interneurons changes during adolescence. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[7]  S. Phillips,et al.  Processing capacity defined by relational complexity: implications for comparative, developmental, and cognitive psychology. , 1998, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[8]  K. Holyoak,et al.  Children's development of analogical reasoning: insights from scene analogy problems. , 2006, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[9]  James K. Kroger,et al.  Rostrolateral Prefrontal Cortex Involvement in Relational Integration during Reasoning , 2001, NeuroImage.

[10]  John D E Gabrieli,et al.  Evaluating self-generated information: anterior prefrontal contributions to human cognition. , 2003, Behavioral neuroscience.

[11]  L. Spear The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations , 2000, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[12]  Carter Wendelken,et al.  Neurocognitive development of relational reasoning. , 2009, Developmental science.

[13]  L. Westlye,et al.  Brain maturation in adolescence and young adulthood: regional age-related changes in cortical thickness and white matter volume and microstructure. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[14]  S. Carey,et al.  Development of face recognition: A maturational component? , 1980 .

[15]  D. Gentner,et al.  More evidence for a relational shift in the development of analogy: Children's performance on a causal-mapping task , 1998 .

[16]  S. Blakemore,et al.  Development of the Selection and Manipulation of Self-Generated Thoughts in Adolescence , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  Dinggang Shen,et al.  Puberty-related influences on brain development , 2006, Molecular and Cellular Endocrinology.

[18]  Judy Reilly,et al.  Cognitive efficiency on a match to sample task decreases at the onset of puberty in children , 2002, Brain and Cognition.

[19]  A. Toga,et al.  In vivo evidence for post-adolescent brain maturation in frontal and striatal regions , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[20]  Eugenio Rodriguez,et al.  The development of neural synchrony reflects late maturation and restructuring of functional networks in humans , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[21]  Kuei Y Tseng,et al.  Post-pubertal emergence of prefrontal cortical up states induced by D1-NMDA co-activation. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[22]  Carter Wendelken,et al.  Left, but not right, rostrolateral prefrontal cortex meets a stringent test of the relational integration hypothesis , 2009, NeuroImage.

[23]  A. Owen,et al.  Anterior prefrontal cortex: insights into function from anatomy and neuroimaging , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[24]  Peter B Barker,et al.  In vivo quantitative proton MRSI study of brain development from childhood to adolescence , 2002, Journal of magnetic resonance imaging : JMRI.

[25]  James K. Kroger,et al.  Recruitment of anterior dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in human reasoning: a parametric study of relational complexity. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[26]  T. Q. Irigaray,et al.  Intellectual abilities in Alzheimer's disease patients: Contributions from the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) , 2010 .

[27]  G. Glover,et al.  Biological Substrates of Emotional Reactivity and Regulation in Adolescence During an Emotional Go-Nogo Task , 2008, Biological Psychiatry.

[28]  Kalina Christoff,et al.  Localizing the rostrolateral prefrontal cortex at the individual level , 2007, NeuroImage.

[29]  K. Christoff,et al.  Prefrontal organization of cognitive control according to levels of abstraction , 2009, Brain Research.

[30]  W. K. Simmons,et al.  Circular analysis in systems neuroscience: the dangers of double dipping , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  Neil G. Muggleton,et al.  Control of prepotent responses by the superior medial frontal cortex , 2009, NeuroImage.

[32]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Voxel-Based Morphometry—The Methods , 2000, NeuroImage.

[33]  V. Menon,et al.  White matter development during childhood and adolescence: a cross-sectional diffusion tensor imaging study. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[34]  Daniel Durstewitz,et al.  Dopamine modulation , 2008, Scholarpedia.

[35]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  The frontopolar cortex and human cognition: Evidence for a rostrocaudal hierarchical organization within the human prefrontal cortex , 2000, Psychobiology.

[36]  R. Sternberg,et al.  The development of analogical reasoning processes. , 1979, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[37]  T. Insel,et al.  The ontogeny of excitatory amino acid receptors in rat forebrain—I.N-methyl-d-aspartate and quisqualate receptors , 1990, Neuroscience.

[38]  E. Koechlin,et al.  The Architecture of Cognitive Control in the Human Prefrontal Cortex , 2003, Science.

[39]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[40]  S. Petersen,et al.  The maturing architecture of the brain's default network , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.