Normal rates of cognitive change in successful aging: The Freedom House Study

We determined the rates of cognitive change associated with twenty individual measures. Participants included 547 noninstitutionalized septuagenarians and octogenarian residents of a comprehensive care retirement community who were studied over three years. Latent growth curves (LGC) of multiple cognitive measures were compared to a LGC model of the rates of change in Activities of Daily Living (ADL) and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living (IADL). All curves were standardized relative to each variable's baseline distribution. Baseline scores were within their expected normal age-specific ranges. Most measures showed significant rates of change over time. There was also significant variability about those rates, suggesting clinical heterogeneity. Many deteriorated over time, as did ADLs and IADLs. However, performance on some measures improved, consistent with learning effects. The rates of change in two measures, the Executive Interview and the Trail Making Test, were closely related to decline in IADLs. These results suggest that age-related cognitive decline is a dynamic longitudinal process affecting multiple cognitive domains. Heterogeneity in the rates of cognitive change may reflect the summed effects of age and comorbid conditions affecting cognition. Some measures may be ill-suited for measuring age-related changes in cognition, either because they are insensitive to change, or hindered by learning effects. Nonverbal measures appear to be particularly well suited for the prediction of age-related functional decline. These observations are relevant to the definition and diagnosis of “dementing” conditions. (JINS, 2005, 11, 899–909.)

[1]  Kiralee M. Hayashi,et al.  Dynamics of Gray Matter Loss in Alzheimer's Disease , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  Jürgen Baumert,et al.  Modeling longitudinal and multilevel data: Practical issues, applied approaches, and specific examples. , 2000 .

[3]  H. Kraemer,et al.  How can we learn about developmental processes from cross-sectional studies, or can we? , 2000, The American journal of psychiatry.

[4]  R. Bornstein,et al.  Practice Effects on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test–64 Card Version Across 12 Months , 2001, The Clinical neuropsychologist.

[5]  Guk-Hee Suh,et al.  A longitudinal study of Alzheimer's disease: rates of cognitive and functional decline , 2004, International journal of geriatric psychiatry.

[6]  S. Allen,et al.  Acquisition and short-term retention of inhaler techniques require intact executive function in elderly subjects. , 2003, Age and ageing.

[7]  Ulman Lindenberger,et al.  Static and Dynamic Longitudinal Structural Analyses of Cognitive Changes in Old Age , 2003, Gerontology.

[8]  J. Goodwin,et al.  Wisconsin Card Sort Test performance in a healthy elderly population. , 1987, Journal of gerontology.

[9]  D R Royall,et al.  Clock drawing is sensitive to executive control: a comparison of six methods. , 1999, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[10]  R. Reitan Validity of the Trail Making Test as an Indicator of Organic Brain Damage , 1958 .

[11]  A. Saykin,et al.  Brain activation on fMRI and verbal memory ability: Functional neuroanatomic correlates of CVLT performance , 2001, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[12]  M. Bobinski,et al.  Frequency of Stages of Alzheimer-Related Lesions in Different Age Categories , 1997, Neurobiology of Aging.

[13]  D Commenges,et al.  A 5-year longitudinal study of the Mini-Mental State Examination in normal aging. , 1997, American journal of epidemiology.

[14]  T. Wise,et al.  Executive Functioning and Compliance in HIV Patients , 1999, Psychological reports.

[15]  D. Royall,et al.  Bedside Assessment of Executive Cognitive Impairment: The Executive Interview , 1992, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[16]  M. Browne,et al.  Alternative Ways of Assessing Model Fit , 1992 .

[17]  J. Touchon,et al.  Disability and mild cognitive impairment: a longitudinal population‐based study , 2001, International journal of geriatric psychiatry.

[18]  J. Baumert,et al.  Longitudinal and multi-group modeling with missing data , 2022 .

[19]  David A. Rettinger,et al.  How are visuospatial working memory, executive functioning, and spatial abilities related? A latent-variable analysis. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[20]  D. Royall,et al.  CLOX: an executive clock drawing task , 1998, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[21]  P. Dastidar,et al.  Unawareness of deficits after right hemisphere stroke: double‐dissociationsof anosognosias , 2000, Acta neurologica Scandinavica.

[22]  A. Saykin,et al.  Functional differentiation of medial temporal and frontal regions involved in processing novel and familiar words: an fMRI study. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[23]  R. Palmer,et al.  Executive Control Mediates Memory's Association with Change in Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: The Freedom House Study , 2005, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[24]  K. Christensen,et al.  The Heritability of Level and Rate-of-Change in Cognitive Functioning in Danish Twins Aged 70 Years and Older , 2002, Experimental aging research.

[25]  G. Maddox Duke University Center for the Study of Aging and Human Development:: Past, Present, and Future , 1993 .

[26]  David E. Booth,et al.  Analysis of Incomplete Multivariate Data , 2000, Technometrics.

[27]  H. Braak,et al.  Evolution of neuronal changes in the course of Alzheimer's disease. , 1998, Journal of neural transmission. Supplementum.

[28]  D. Royall,et al.  Misclassification Is Likely in the Assessment of Mild Cognitive Impairment , 2004, Neuroepidemiology.

[29]  John B. Willett,et al.  Using covariance structure analysis to detect correlates and predictors of individual change over time , 1994 .

[30]  Emilio Ferrer,et al.  Modeling age and retest processes in longitudinal studies of cognitive abilities. , 2004, Psychology and aging.

[31]  H. Tuokko,et al.  A comparison of methods for measuring cognitive change in older adults. , 2005, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[32]  D. Royall,et al.  Frontal MRI Findings Associated with Impairment on the Executive Interview (EXIT25) , 2001, Experimental aging research.

[33]  A. Delacourte,et al.  Pathological Determinants of the Transition to Clinical Dementia in Alzheimer's Disease , 2002, Experimental aging research.

[34]  D P Salmon,et al.  A normative study of Nelson's (1976) modified version of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in healthy older adults. , 1999, The Clinical neuropsychologist.

[35]  E. Kaplan,et al.  The 'preclinical phase' of probable Alzheimer's disease. A 13-year prospective study of the Framingham cohort. , 1995, Archives of neurology.

[36]  D. Royall,et al.  Executive Dyscontrol: An Important Factor Affecting the Level of Care Received by Older Retirees , 1998, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[37]  D. Royall,et al.  Dementias That Present With and Without Posterior Cortical Features: An Important Clinical Distinction , 1998, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[38]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.

[39]  D. Royall,et al.  Correlates of disability among elderly retirees with "subclinical" cognitive impairment. , 2000, The journals of gerontology. Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences.

[40]  Charles Hall,et al.  Modeling memory decline in older adults: the importance of preclinical dementia, attrition, and chronological age. , 2003, Psychology and aging.

[41]  P. Baltes,et al.  The fate of cognition in very old age: six-year longitudinal findings in the Berlin Aging Study (BASE). , 2003, Psychology and aging.

[42]  P. Bentler,et al.  Comparative fit indexes in structural models. , 1990, Psychological bulletin.

[43]  James L. Arbuckle,et al.  Full Information Estimation in the Presence of Incomplete Data , 1996 .

[44]  S. Rapp,et al.  Baseline experience with Modified Mini Mental State Exam: The Women's Health Initiative Memory Study (WHIMS) , 2003, Aging & mental health.

[45]  J. Yesavage,et al.  Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS): Recent evidence and development of a shorter version. , 1986 .

[46]  Hannie C. Comijs,et al.  The Course of Cognitive Decline in Older Persons: Results from the Longitudinal Aging Study Amsterdam , 2004, Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders.

[47]  E. Kisch,et al.  Changes in serum dopamine-beta-hydroxylase activity during cold pressor test in subjects with high and low basal activity of this enzyme , 2005, Journal of Neural Transmission.

[48]  J J McArdle,et al.  Modeling incomplete longitudinal and cross-sectional data using latent growth structural models. , 1992, Experimental aging research.

[49]  G. Ratcliff,et al.  Tracking Cognitive Functioning Over Time: Ten-Year Longitudinal Data From a Community-Based Study , 2003, Applied neuropsychology.

[50]  Sudha Seshadri,et al.  Framingham Stroke Risk Profile and Lowered Cognitive Performance , 2004, Stroke.

[51]  R. Petersen,et al.  Mild cognitive impairment , 2006, The Lancet.

[52]  R. Bornstein,et al.  Practice effects on commonly used measures of executive function across twelve months. , 1999, The Clinical neuropsychologist.

[53]  J. Morris,et al.  Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. , 2001, Archives of neurology.

[54]  Robert K. Heaton,et al.  Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual – Revised and Expanded , 1993 .

[55]  W. Burke,et al.  A Comparison of Two Depression Scales in a Geriatric Assessment Clinic. , 1995, The American journal of geriatric psychiatry : official journal of the American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry.

[56]  N. Pedersen,et al.  Sources of Influence on Rate of Cognitive Change Over Time in Swedish Twins: An Application of Latent Growth Models , 2002, Experimental aging research.

[57]  Christopher Hertzog,et al.  Latent change models of adult cognition: are changes in processing speed and working memory associated with changes in episodic memory? , 2003, Psychology and aging.

[58]  E. Tangalos,et al.  Mild Cognitive Impairment Clinical Characterization and Outcome , 1999 .

[59]  R. Palmer,et al.  Declining Executive Control in Normal Aging Predicts Change in Functional Status: The Freedom House Study , 2004, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[60]  D. Royall “Silent stroke”: An oxymoron meaning “dementia” , 2004 .

[61]  Alexander L. Miller,et al.  Executive impairment among the functionally dependent: comparisons between schizophrenic and elderly subjects. , 1993, The American journal of psychiatry.

[62]  R. Cranston Competency to consent to medical treatment in cognitively impaired patients with Parkinson’s disease , 2001, Neurology.

[63]  D. Royall,et al.  Executive control and the comprehension of medical information by elderly retirees. , 1997, Experimental aging research.

[64]  J. S. Long,et al.  Testing Structural Equation Models , 1993 .

[65]  D. Royall,et al.  Executive dyscontrol in normal aging: Normative data, factor structure, and clinical correlates , 2003, Current neurology and neuroscience reports.

[66]  E. Benarroch Executive Control , 2022, Neuroscience for Clinicians.

[67]  Kaarin J Anstey,et al.  A latent growth curve analysis of late-life sensory and cognitive function over 8 years: evidence for specific and common factors underlying change. , 2003, Psychology and aging.