Locomotor Ability and Wariness in Yellow‐Bellied Marmots
暂无分享,去创建一个
Janice C. Daniel | D. Blumstein | A. Ozgul | Andrea M. Runyan | Mona Seymour | A. Nicodemus | F. Ransler | Soyeon Im | Tricia Stark | Claire A. Zugmeyer | J. C. Daniel
[1] D. Falconer,et al. Introduction to Quantitative Genetics. , 1962 .
[2] Jacob Cohen. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.
[3] I. Vine,et al. Risk of visual detection and pursuitby a predator and the selective advantage of flocking behaviour. , 1971, Journal of theoretical biology.
[4] W. Hamilton. Geometry for the selfish herd. , 1971, Journal of theoretical biology.
[5] C. R. Taylor,et al. Running Up and Down Hills: Some Consequences of Size , 1972, Science.
[6] D. Blanchard,et al. Effects of hippocampal lesions on the rat's reaction to a cat. , 1972, Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology.
[7] H. Pulliam,et al. On the advantages of flocking. , 1973, Journal of theoretical biology.
[8] Pulliam Hr,et al. On the advantages of flocking , 1973 .
[9] K. Armitage. Social behavior and population dynamics of marmots , 1975 .
[10] W. Atchley,et al. Statistical Properties of Ratios. I. Empirical Results , 1976 .
[11] Predation by Badger on Yellow-Bellied Marmot in Colorado , 1977 .
[12] S. Gould,et al. The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: a critique of the adaptationist programme , 1979, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.
[13] D. Leger,et al. Behavioral ecology of time allocation in California ground squirrels (Spermophilus beecheyi): Microhabitat effects. , 1983 .
[14] E. Werner,et al. THE ONTOGENETIC NICHE AND SPECIES INTERACTIONS IN SIZE-STRUCTURED POPULATIONS , 1984 .
[15] P. Moore,et al. Foraging and Predation Risk in Yellow-Bellied Marmots , 1986 .
[16] Lawrence M. Dill,et al. The Economics of Fleeing from Predators , 1986 .
[17] C. Clark,et al. The evolutionary advantages of group foraging , 1986 .
[18] K. Armitage. Social dynamics of mammals: Reproductive success, kinship and individual fitness. , 1987, Trends in ecology & evolution.
[19] S. H. Jenkins,et al. Sex and age affect vulnerability of desert rodents to owl predation , 1987 .
[20] Theodore Garland,et al. Maximal Running Speeds of Bipedal and Quadrupedal Rodents , 1988 .
[21] T. Caraco,et al. Effects of predation hazard on foraging "constraints": patch-use strategies in grey squirrels , 1988 .
[22] T. Schoener. On Testing the MacArthur-Wilson Model with Data on Rates , 1988, The American Naturalist.
[23] K. Armitage. Chapter 7 – RESOURCES AND SOCIAL ORGANIZATION OF GROUND-DWELLING SQUIRRELS , 1988 .
[24] Running Speed and Body Mass in Belding's Ground Squirrels , 1989 .
[25] M. Elgar,et al. PREDATOR VIGILANCE AND GROUP SIZE IN MAMMALS AND BIRDS: A CRITICAL REVIEW OF THE EMPIRICAL EVIDENCE , 1989, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.
[26] L. Dill,et al. The influence of distance to refuge on flight initiation distance in the gray squirrel (Sciurus carolinensis) , 1989 .
[27] E. Brodie,et al. Genetic correlations between morphology and antipredator behaviour in natural populations of the garter snake Thamnophis ordinoides , 1989, Nature.
[28] S. L. Lima,et al. Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus , 1990 .
[29] J. Losos,et al. THE EVOLUTION OF FORM AND FUNCTION: MORPHOLOGY AND LOCOMOTOR PERFORMANCE IN WEST INDIAN ANOLIS LIZARDS , 1990, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.
[30] M. Cassini,et al. Foraging under predation risk in the wild guinea pig Cavia aperea , 1991 .
[31] D. V. Vuren. Yellow-bellied Marmots as Prey of Coyotes , 1991 .
[32] D. Blumstein. MULTIVARIATE ANALYSIS OF GOLDEN MARMOT MAXIMUM RUNNING SPEED: A NEW METHOD TO STUDY MRS IN THE FIELD' , 1992 .
[33] Joel s. Brown,et al. Patch use predation risk. II : A test with fox squirrels, Sciurus niger , 1992 .
[34] S. Riechert,et al. A test for correlations among fitness-linked behavioural traits in the spider Agelenopsis aperta (Araneae, Agelenidae) , 1993, Animal Behaviour.
[35] M. Bowers,et al. Variation in giving-up densities of foraging chipmunks (Tamias striatus) and squirrels (Sciurus carolinensis) , 1993 .
[36] Andreas G. Lazari,et al. The ontogeny of individuality in black-tailed prairie dogs, Cynomys ludovicianus , 1994 .
[37] A. Magurran,et al. Predator Inspection Behaviour Covaries With Schooling Tendency Amongst Wild Guppy, Poecilia Reticulata, Populations in Trinidad , 1994 .
[38] J. Endler. Multiple-trait coevolution and environmental gradients in guppies. , 1995, Trends in ecology & evolution.
[39] Roger Catchpole,et al. Quantitative Genetic Studies of Behavioral Evolution , 1995 .
[40] J. Losos,et al. The effect of perch diameter on escape behaviour of Anolis lizards : laboratory predictions and field tests , 1996, Animal Behaviour.
[41] K. Armitage,et al. Population time budget for the yellow-bellied marmot , 1996 .
[42] D. Blumstein. How Much Does Social Group Size Influence Golden Marmot Vigilance , 1996 .
[43] D. Kramer,et al. The influence of distance to burrow on flight initiation distance in the woodchuck, Marmota monax , 1996 .
[44] Age differences in the response of California ground Squirrels (Spermophilus beecheyi) to avian and mammalian predators. , 1997, Journal of comparative psychology.
[45] D. Blanchard. Stimulus, environmental, and pharmacological control of defensive behaviors. , 1997 .
[46] D. Leger,et al. Artificial visual obstruction, antipredator vigilance, and predator detection in the thirteen-lined ground squirrel (Spermophilus tridecemlineatus) , 1997 .
[47] B. V. Horne,et al. Influence of habitat on behavior of townsend's ground squirrels (Spermophilus townsendii) , 1998 .
[48] K. Armitage,et al. Life history consequences of social complexity a comparative study of ground-dwelling sciurids , 1998 .
[49] J. Byers. American Pronghorn: Social Adaptations and the Ghosts of Predators Past , 1998 .
[50] S. L. Lima,et al. Back to the basics of antipredatory vigilance: can nonvigilant animals detect attack? , 1999, Animal Behaviour.
[51] J. Losos,et al. Do Lizards Avoid Habitats in Which Performance Is Submaximal? The Relationship between Sprinting Capabilities and Structural Habitat Use in Caribbean Anoles , 1999, The American Naturalist.
[52] J. Capitanio. Personality dimensions in adult male rhesus macaques: Prediction of behaviors across time and situation , 1999, American journal of primatology.
[53] M. Low,et al. Dopamine D4 Receptor-Knock-Out Mice Exhibit Reduced Exploration of Novel Stimuli , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[54] R Hen,et al. 5-HT1B Receptor Knock-Out Mice Exhibit Increased Exploratory Activity and Enhanced Spatial Memory Performance in the Morris Water Maze , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[55] A. Hedrick. Crickets with extravagant mating songs compensate for predation risk with extra caution , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[56] A. Kamil,et al. The cost of limited attention in blue jays , 2000 .
[57] G. Griebel,et al. Mouse defensive behaviors: pharmacological and behavioral assays for anxiety and panic , 2001, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[58] Pilar López,et al. Camouflage and escape decisions in the common chameleon Chamaeleo chamaeleon , 2001 .
[59] D. V. Vuren. Predation on Yellow-bellied Marmots (Marmota flaviventris) , 2001 .
[60] S. Gosling. From mice to men: what can we learn about personality from animal research? , 2001, Psychological bulletin.
[61] Janice C. Daniel,et al. Isolation from mammalian predators differentially affects two congeners , 2002 .
[62] Laurie M. Kauffman,et al. Are vigilance, risk from avian predators and group size consequences of habitat structure? a comparison of three species of squirrel monkey (Saimiri oerstedii, S. boliviensis, and S. sciureus) , 2003 .
[63] A. Sih,et al. Behavioural correlations across situations and the evolution of antipredator behaviour in a sunfish–salamander system , 2003, Animal Behaviour.
[64] Menna E. Jones,et al. Structurally complex habitat and sensory adaptations mediate the behavioural responses of a desert rodent to an indirect cue for increased predation risk , 2003 .
[65] D. Blumstein,et al. Do fences protect birds from human disturbance , 2003 .
[66] D. Irschick. Measuring Performance in Nature: Implications for Studies of Fitness Within Populations1 , 2003, Integrative and comparative biology.
[67] D. Blumstein. Flight-initiation distance in birds is dependent on intruder starting distance , 2003 .
[68] J. Melville,et al. Evolutionary correlations between escape behaviour and performance ability in eight species of snow skinks (Niveoscincus: Lygosominae) from Tasmania , 2003 .
[69] A. Sih,et al. Behavioral Syndromes: An Integrative Overview , 2004, The Quarterly Review of Biology.
[70] D. Schluter,et al. The Genetic Architecture of Parallel Armor Plate Reduction in Threespine Sticklebacks , 2004, PLoS biology.
[71] W. Cooper,et al. Locomotor impairment and defense in gravid lizards (Eumeces laticeps): behavioral shift in activity may offset costs of reproduction in an active forager , 1990, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[72] L. Dill. Distance-to-cover and the escape decisions of an African cichlid fish, Melanochromis chipokae , 1990, Environmental Biology of Fishes.
[73] D. Blumstein,et al. DO INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES INFLUENCE FLIGHT INITIATION DISTANCE? , 2004 .
[74] Michael M. Delm. Vigilance for predators: detection and dilution effects , 1990, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[75] C. Boake. Repeatability: Its role in evolutionary studies of mating behavior , 1989, Evolutionary Ecology.