Multinational investigation of cross-societal cooperation

Significance We present a comprehensive analysis of cross-societal cooperation involving full incentivization and population-representative samples of participants from Germany, India, Israel, Japan, Mexico, and the United States. Our main finding is that individuals have shared stereotypes in terms of expected cooperation for interaction partners from different nations. Individuals also hold (shared) social preferences toward these partners, which are driven by ingroup favoritism, differences in wealth, and additional factors. We discuss our results with respect to theories that explain cooperation behavior by similarity, inequality aversion, and specific expectations. In a globalized world, establishing successful cooperation between people from different nations is becoming increasingly important. We present results from a comprehensive investigation of cross-societal cooperation in one-shot prisoner’s dilemmas involving population-representative samples from six countries and identify crucial facilitators of and obstacles to cooperation. In interactions involving mutual knowledge about only the other players’ nationalities, we demonstrate that people hold strong and transnationally shared expectations (i.e., stereotypes) concerning the cooperation level of interaction partners from other countries. These expectations are the strongest determinants of participant cooperation. Paradoxically, however, they turn out to be incorrect stereotypes that even correlate negatively with reality. In addition to erroneous expectations, participants’ cooperation behavior is driven by (shared) social preferences that vary according to the interaction partner’s nationality. In the cross-societal context, these social preferences are influenced by differences in wealth and ingroup favoritism, as well as effects of specific country combinations but not by spatial distance between nations.

[1]  P. V. Lange,et al.  The pursuit of joint outcomes and equality in outcomes: An integrative model of social value orientation. , 1999 .

[2]  R. Boyd,et al.  In Search of Homo Economicus: Behavioral Experiments in 15 Small- Scale Societies , 2001 .

[3]  C. Fershtman,et al.  Discrimination and Nepotism: The Efficiency of the Anonymity Rule , 2002, The Journal of Legal Studies.

[4]  L. Jussim,et al.  Ethnic and National Stereotypes: The Princeton Trilogy Revisited and Revised , 2001 .

[5]  Michael J. Platow,et al.  Comparisons of Australians and Japanese on group‐based cooperation , 2005 .

[6]  Amy J. C. Cuddy,et al.  The BIAS map: behaviors from intergroup affect and stereotypes. , 2007, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[7]  Urs Fischbacher,et al.  The behavioral validity of the strategy method in public good experiments , 2012 .

[8]  D.,et al.  THE EVOLUTION OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR , 2002 .

[9]  A. Glöckner,et al.  Personality and prosocial behavior: linking basic traits and social value orientations. , 2014, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[10]  Daniel Balliet,et al.  Ingroup favoritism in cooperation: a meta-analysis. , 2014, Psychological bulletin.

[11]  Roderick M. Kramer,et al.  Choice behavior in social dilemmas: Effects of social identity, group size, and decision framing. , 1986 .

[12]  R. McCrae,et al.  National Character and Personality , 2006 .

[13]  B. Hewlett,et al.  Ontogeny of prosocial behavior across diverse societies , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[14]  Enrique Fatas,et al.  Globalization and human cooperation , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[15]  D. Hamilton Conceptual Approaches to Stereotypes and Stereotyping , 2015 .

[16]  Armin Falk,et al.  Do Lab Experiments Misrepresent Social Preferences? The Case of Self-Selected Student Samples , 2013 .

[17]  Wolfgang J. Luhan,et al.  Cedex Discussion Paper Series , 2022 .

[18]  Sebastian J. Goerg,et al.  Fusing enacted and expected mimicry generates a winning strategy that promotes the evolution of cooperation , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[19]  James H. Liu,et al.  Unbalanced triangle in the social dilemma of trust: Internet studies of real-time, real money social exchange between China, Japan, and Taiwan: Trust in internet social exchange , 2011 .

[20]  J. Henrich,et al.  Culture–gene coevolution, norm-psychology and the emergence of human prosociality , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[21]  R. Selten,et al.  Actions and Beliefs in a Trilateral Trust Game Involving Germans. Israelis and Palestinians , 2007 .

[22]  E. Fehr A Theory of Fairness, Competition and Cooperation , 1998 .

[23]  Amy J. C. Cuddy,et al.  Stereotype content model across cultures: towards universal similarities and some differences. , 2009, The British journal of social psychology.

[24]  Melvin J. Kimmel,et al.  Twenty Years of Experimental Gaming: Critique,Synthesis, and Suggestions for the Future , 1977 .

[25]  James H. Fowler,et al.  Egalitarian motives in humans , 2007, Nature.

[26]  David F. Sally Conversation and Cooperation in Social Dilemmas , 1995 .

[27]  Amy J. C. Cuddy,et al.  A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[28]  Albert Costa,et al.  “Piensa” twice: On the foreign language effect in decision making , 2014, Cognition.

[29]  H. Tajfel Social identity and intergroup behaviour , 1974 .

[30]  Amy J. C. Cuddy,et al.  Universal dimensions of social cognition: warmth and competence , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[31]  Christophe Boone,et al.  Social value orientation and cooperation in social dilemmas: a review and conceptual model. , 2008, The British journal of social psychology.

[32]  G. Hofstede,et al.  Cultures and Organizations: Software of the Mind , 1991 .

[33]  G. M. Gilbert Stereotype persistence and change among college students. , 1951, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[34]  David M. Kreps,et al.  Rational cooperation in the finitely repeated prisoners' dilemma , 1982 .

[35]  Eric J. Johnson,et al.  Let's Get Personal: An International Examination of the Influence of Communication, Culture and Social Distance on Other Regarding Preferences , 2006 .

[36]  Gert Jan Hofstede,et al.  Cultures and Organizations: Software of the Mind, 3rd ed. , 2010 .

[37]  S. Levinson,et al.  WEIRD languages have misled us, too , 2010, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[38]  C. Barbaranelli,et al.  National Character Does Not Reflect Mean Personality Trait Levels in 49 Cultures , 2005, Science.

[39]  G. Hardin,et al.  The Tragedy of the Commons , 1968, Green Planet Blues.

[40]  C. B. Colby The weirdest people in the world , 1973 .

[41]  Feixue Wang,et al.  The intercultural trust paradigm: Studying joint cultural interaction and social exchange in real time over the Internet , 2008 .

[42]  Simon Gächter,et al.  Reciprocity, culture and human cooperation: previous insights and a new cross-cultural experiment , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[43]  T. Hedden,et al.  What do you think I think you think?: Strategic reasoning in matrix games , 2002, Cognition.

[44]  C. Fershtman,et al.  Trust and discrimination in a segmented society: An experimental approach , 2001 .

[45]  Gary E. Bolton,et al.  ERC: A Theory of Equity, Reciprocity, and Competition , 2000 .

[46]  L M Wahl,et al.  The Continuous Prisoner:s Dilemma: I. Linear Reactive Strategies , 1999 .

[47]  S. Gächter,et al.  Culture and cooperation , 2010, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.