Personality, reaction time, and event-related potentials.

Reaction time (RT), movement time (MT), and the amplitude and latency of the P300 event-related potential were recorded from 30 Ss during the performance of 6 simple cognitive tasks. Extraversion was negatively associated with MT, a result that endorses the view that extraversion is determined, in part, by individual differences in motor mechanisms. Higher neuroticism scores were associated with faster P300 latency, a measure that is regarded as an index of stimulus evaluation time that is independent of response production. Paradoxically, higher neuroticism scores were associated with slower RT, a measure that is also regarded as an index of speed of information processing. Higher psychoticism scores were associated with smaller P300 amplitude, an effect that may be indicative of less attentional effort invested in the tasks. The analyses of individual differences in the response to sensory stimulation and in the expression of motor responses are important sources of evidence for several proposals that have been put forward in the explanation of personality differences, notably extraversion, impulsiveness, and anxiety (Barratt, 1972; Brebner & Cooper, 1974; H. J. Eysenck, 1957; Newman, 1987; Stelmack, 1985). The present article aims to articulate differences in the processing of stimuli and production of responses that are relevant to the biological bases of those personality dimensions by exploiting relatively recent refinements in eventrelated potential (ERP) and reaction time (RT) measures that are recorded during the performance of simple cognitive tasks. This article addresses several questions that have emerged in the study of individual differences in extraversion and impulsiveness. At this time, there is a good deal of evidence from research using both behavioral and psychophysiological measures that demonstrates the greater sensory reaction to simple physical stimulation for introverts than extraverts (Stelmack, 1990). However, it is unclear whether these effects reflect differences at early stages of stimulus input, central stimulus analysis, or both. There is also a literature spanning 30 years that affirms differences between extraverts and introverts and between high- and low-impulsive subjects in their performance on various tasks that assess speed of responding and motor control (Barratt & Patton, 1983; Stelmack, 1985). Again, it is not clear whether these effects involve differences in central processing, motor response, or both. This issue is complicated because impulsiveness can be described as a complex construct composed of facets of extraversion, neuroticism, and psychoticism (H. J.

[1]  J. Brebner,et al.  The effect of catch-trials on speed and accuracy among introverts and extraverts in a simple RT task. , 1978, British journal of psychology.

[2]  J. H. Daruna,et al.  Introversion, attention and the late positive component of event-related potentials , 1985, Biological Psychology.

[3]  D. Kosson,et al.  Reaction to punishment, reflectivity, and passive avoidance learning in extraverts. , 1987, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[4]  C. Frith,et al.  Strategies in rotary pursuit tracking. , 1971, British journal of psychology.

[5]  D. Fowles,et al.  The three arousal model: implications of gray's two-factor learning theory for heart rate, electrodermal activity, and psychopathy. , 1980, Psychophysiology.

[6]  P. Kohn Issues in the Measurement of Arousability , 1987 .

[7]  P. Costa,et al.  Facet Scales for Agreeableness and Conscientiousness: A Revision of the NEO Personality Inventory☆ , 1991 .

[8]  P. Vernon Speed of Information Processing and General Intelligence. , 1983 .

[9]  M. J. Lynch,et al.  Biological Bases of Sensation Seeking, Impulsivity, and Anxiety , 1984 .

[10]  R. Stelmack Personality and motor activity: a psychophysiological perspective , 1985 .

[11]  E. P. Gorsel EEG and cardiac correlates of neuroticism: A psychophysiological comparison of neurotics and normal controls in relation to personality , 1981, Biological Psychology.

[12]  H. Eysenck,et al.  Sport and personality , 1982 .

[13]  J. Brebner Personality theory and movement , 1985 .

[14]  T. G. Reeve,et al.  Correlations of movement time and intelligence: Effects of simplifying response requirements , 1990 .

[15]  John E. Laird,et al.  Stimulus-Response Compatibility , 1986 .

[16]  K Gilliland,et al.  Eysenck's arousal theory of introversion-extraversion: a converging measures investigation. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[17]  B. Kopell,et al.  The Stroop effect: brain potentials localize the source of interference. , 1981, Science.

[18]  W. Pritchard P300 and EPQ/STPI personality traits☆ , 1989 .

[19]  N L Segal,et al.  Personality similarity in twins reared apart and together. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[20]  E. Donchin,et al.  On quantifying surprise: the variation of event-related potentials with subjective probability. , 1977, Psychophysiology.

[21]  Ingmar Rosén,et al.  ATTENTION AND PERSONALITY IN AUGMENTING/ REDUCING OF VISUAL EVOKED POTENTIALS , 1990 .

[22]  H. Eysenck,et al.  Dynamics of Anxiety and Hysteria , 1957 .

[23]  John Wallace,et al.  Failures of response modulation: Impulsive behavior in anxious and impulsive individuals , 1991 .

[24]  J. Newman,et al.  Reaction to punishment in extraverts and psychopaths: Implications for the impulsive behavior of disinhibited individuals , 1987 .

[25]  R. Stelmack The Psychophysiology of Extraversion and Neuroticism , 1981 .

[26]  R. Geen,et al.  Preferred stimulation levels in introverts and extroverts: Effects on arousal and performance. , 1984 .

[27]  H. Eysenck,et al.  Psychoticism as a dimension of personality , 1976 .

[28]  John W. Rohrbaugh,et al.  13 Sensory and Motor Aspects of the Contingent Negative Variation , 1983 .

[29]  J. Polich,et al.  P300 and introverted/extraverted personality types. , 1991, Psychophysiology.

[30]  K. O'Connor Individual differences in components of slow cortical potentials: implications for models of information processing , 1983 .

[31]  E. Donchin,et al.  Cognitive Psychophysiology and Human Information Processing , 1986 .

[32]  H. J. Eysenck,et al.  Reminiscence, Motivation, and Personality , 1977 .

[33]  E. Barratt,et al.  Anxiety and Impulsiveness Related to Psychomotor Efficiency , 1959 .

[34]  D. Schalling Tolerance for experimentally induced pain as related to personality. , 1971, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[35]  H. Eysenck,et al.  Personality Structure and Measurement , 1968 .

[36]  K. Campbell,et al.  Neuroanatomical and physiological foundations of extraversion. , 1981, Psychophysiology.

[37]  S. Nichols,et al.  Effects of punishment on response latency in extraverts. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[38]  Terence W. Picton,et al.  Human event-related potentials , 1988 .

[39]  R. Stelmack,et al.  Extraversion and individual differences in auditory evoked response. , 1977, Psychophysiology.

[40]  David M. Diamond,et al.  Applications of event-related brain potentials to problems in engineering psychology , 1986 .

[41]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Augmenting mental chronometry: the P300 as a measure of stimulus evaluation time. , 1977, Science.

[42]  R. Taft Extraversion, neuroticism, and expressive behavior: an application of Wallach's moderator effect to handwriting analysis. , 1967, Journal of personality.

[43]  B. Kirkcaldy,et al.  Individual Differences in Movement , 1985, Springer Netherlands.

[44]  J C Loehlin,et al.  Partitioning environmental and genetic contributions to behavioral development. , 1989, The American psychologist.

[45]  H. Eysenck A Model for Personality , 1981 .

[46]  Susan G. Campbell,et al.  Regression and criterion measures of habituation: A comparative analysis in extraverts and introverts , 1990 .

[47]  R. Stelmack,et al.  Extraversion and the effects of frequency and intensity on the auditory brainstem evoked response , 1982 .

[48]  R. T. Pivik,et al.  Personality and individual differences in spinal motoneuronal excitability. , 1988, Psychophysiology.

[49]  J. Polich,et al.  P300, cognitive capability, and personality: A correlational study of university undergraduates , 1992 .

[50]  H. Eysenck,et al.  The place of impulsiveness in a dimensional system of personality description. , 1977, The British journal of social and clinical psychology.

[51]  P. Tueting,et al.  Event-related brain potentials in man , 1978 .

[52]  W. Pritchard Psychophysiology of P300. , 1981, Psychological bulletin.

[53]  Hans J. Eysenck,et al.  Personality dimensions and arousal , 1987 .

[54]  E. Barratt Perceptual-Motor Performance Related to Impulsiveness and Anxiety , 1967, Perceptual and motor skills.

[55]  R. Stelmack Biological bases of extraversion: psychophysiological evidence. , 1990, Journal of personality.

[56]  Hans J. Eysenck,et al.  Manual of the Eysenck personality questionnaire , 1975 .

[57]  A. Jensen,et al.  Reaction Time, Movement Time, and Intelligence , 1979 .

[58]  R. Hockey Stress and fatigue in human performance , 1984 .

[59]  Kenneth B. Campbell,et al.  Intelligence, reaction time, and event-related potentials , 1992 .

[60]  E. Donchin,et al.  COGNITIVE PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGY: THE ENDOGENOUS COMPONENTS OF THE ERP , 1978 .

[61]  J. Brebner,et al.  The effect of a low rate of regular signals upon the reaction times of introverts and extraverts , 1974 .

[62]  S. Dornič,et al.  Extraversion, neuroticism, and noise sensitivity , 1990 .

[63]  R. Stelmack,et al.  Introversion-extraversion: the Bell-Magendie law revisited , 1983 .

[64]  A. Gale,et al.  “Stimulus hunger”: Individual differences in operant strategy in a button-pressing task , 1969 .

[65]  F. Farley,et al.  Individual differences in solution time in error-free problem solving. , 1966, The British journal of social and clinical psychology.

[66]  W. Pritchard,et al.  Cognitive event-related potential correlates of schizophrenia. , 1986, Psychological bulletin.

[67]  K. Campbell,et al.  Extraversion and Auditory Sensitivity to High and Low Frequency , 1974, Perceptual and motor skills.

[68]  R. Geen,et al.  The psychophysiology of extraversion. , 1992 .

[69]  E. Barratt,et al.  Chapter 5 – ANXIETY AND IMPULSIVENESS: TOWARD A NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL MODEL1 , 1972 .

[70]  M. Zuckerman Sensation seeking and sports , 1983 .

[71]  K. Campbell,et al.  Extraversion and brainstem auditory evoked potentials during sleep and wakefulness , 1993 .

[72]  Henk A. L. Kiers,et al.  Five (or three) robust questionnaire scale factors of personality without culture , 1991 .

[73]  M. Wallach,et al.  Effects of Anxiety Level and Extraversion-Introversion on Probability Learning , 1960 .

[74]  H. Eysenck Reminiscence as a factor in the learning of skilled movements , 1985 .

[75]  J. Bachorowski,et al.  Impulsive motor behavior: effects of personality and goal salience. , 1990, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[76]  H. Eysenck,et al.  Impulsiveness and Venturesomeness: Their Position in a Dimensional System of Personality Description , 1978, Psychological reports.

[77]  J. Brebner,et al.  Stimulus- or response-induced excitation. A comparison of the behavior of introverts and extraverts , 1978 .

[78]  J. Brebner Psychological and Neurophysiological Factors in Stimulus-Response Compatibility , 1990 .

[79]  J. Polich,et al.  P300, probability, and introverted/extroverted personality types , 1992, Biological Psychology.

[80]  W. Szelenberger Brain stem auditory evoked potentials and personality. , 1983, Biological psychiatry.

[81]  H. Eysenck Biological Basis of Personality , 1963, Nature.

[82]  Motoneuronal insensitivity in extraverts as revealed by the startle response paradigm , 1991 .

[83]  Barry D. Smith Extraversion and electrodermal activity: arousability and the inverted-U , 1983 .

[84]  Michael W. Eysenck,et al.  Handbook of individual differences : biological perspectives , 1992 .

[85]  J. Brebner A Model Of Extraversion , 1983 .

[86]  Charles D. Spielberger,et al.  Anxiety: Current Trends in Theory and Research , 1972 .