Abstract sequential task control is facilitated by practice and embedded motor sequences

Everyday task sequences, such as cooking, contain overarching goals (completing the meal), sub-goals (prepare vegetables), and motor actions (chopping). Such tasks generally are considered hierarchical because superordinate levels (e.g., goals) affect performance at subordinate levels (e.g., sub-goals and motor actions). However, there is debate as to whether this hierarchy is “strict” with unidirectional, top-down influences, and it is unknown if and how practice affects performance at the superordinate levels. To investigate these questions, we manipulated practice with sequences at the goal and motor action levels using an abstract, or non-motor, task sequence paradigm (Desrochers et al., 2015; Schneider & Logan, 2006). In three experiments, participants performed memorized abstract task sequences composed of simple tasks (e.g., color/shape judgements), where some contained embedded motor response sequences. We found that practice facilitated performance and reduced control costs for abstract task sequences and subordinate tasks. The interrelation was different between the hierarchical levels, demonstrating a strict relationship between abstract task sequence goals and sub-goals and a non-strict relationship between sub-goal and motor response levels. Under some conditions, the motor response level influenced the abstract task sequence level in a non-strict manner. Further, manipulating the presence or absence of a motor sequence after learning indicated that these effects were not the result of an integrated representation produced by practice. These experiments provide evidence for a mixed hierarchical model of task sequences and insight into the distinct roles of practice and motor processing in efficiently executing task sequences in daily life.

[1]  James B. Rowe,et al.  Action selection: A race model for selected and non-selected actions distinguishes the contribution of premotor and prefrontal areas , 2010, NeuroImage.

[2]  Atsushi Kikumoto,et al.  Conjunctive representations that integrate stimuli, responses, and rules are critical for action selection , 2019, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[3]  Fang Zhao,et al.  Implicit sequence learning despite multitasking: the role of across-task predictability , 2019, Psychological research.

[4]  Jörn Diedrichsen,et al.  Effector-Independent Motor Sequence Representations Exist in Extrinsic and Intrinsic Reference Frames , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  Herbert Heuer,et al.  Task integration as a factor in secondary-task effects on sequence learning , 1997 .

[6]  C. Stern,et al.  An fMRI Study of the Role of the Medial Temporal Lobe in Implicit and Explicit Sequence Learning , 2003, Neuron.

[7]  G. Dreisbach,et al.  When less is more: costs and benefits of varied vs. fixed content and structure in short-term task switching training , 2019, Psychological research.

[8]  Iring Koch,et al.  Common Cognitive Control Processes Underlying Performance in Task-Switching and Dual-Task Contexts , 2018, Advances in cognitive psychology.

[9]  Franziska M. Korb,et al.  Hierarchically Organized Medial Frontal Cortex-Basal Ganglia Loops Selectively Control Task- and Response-Selection , 2017, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[10]  B. Postle,et al.  The cognitive neuroscience of working memory. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.

[11]  Mike Wendt,et al.  Editorial: Multitasking: Executive Functioning in Dual-Task and Task Switching Situations , 2018, Front. Psychol..

[12]  U. Mayr,et al.  Sticky rules: integration between abstract rules and specific actions. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[13]  Torsten Schubert,et al.  Task switching: effects of practice on switch and mixing costs , 2011, Psychological Research.

[14]  E. Soetens,et al.  Negative priming in the SRT task: Learning of irrelevant sequences is enhanced by concurrent learning of relevant sequences , 2008 .

[15]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[16]  B. Meier,et al.  Implicit Sequence Learning , 2012 .

[17]  R. D. Gordon,et al.  Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. , 2001, Psychological review.

[18]  Hillary D. Schwarb,et al.  Generalized lessons about sequence learning from the study of the serial reaction time task , 2012, Advances in cognitive psychology.

[19]  M. Nissen,et al.  Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.

[20]  Beat Meier,et al.  What matters in implicit task sequence learning: perceptual stimulus features, task sets, or correlated streams of information? , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[21]  T Kleinsorge,et al.  Implicit learning of sequences of tasks. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[22]  B. Meier,et al.  Implicit task sequence learning with auditory stimuli , 2012 .

[23]  Hillary D. Schwarb,et al.  Parallel response selection disrupts sequence learning under dual-task conditions. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[24]  Theresa M. Desrochers,et al.  Sequential Control Underlies Robust Ramping Dynamics in the Rostrolateral Prefrontal Cortex , 2018, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  John W. Krakauer,et al.  Why Are Sequence Representations in Primary Motor Cortex So Elusive? , 2019, Neuron.

[26]  Aaron L. Wong,et al.  Explicit knowledge enhances motor vigor and performance: motivation versus practice in sequence tasks. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  Jörn Diedrichsen,et al.  Neural Organization of Hierarchical Motor Sequence Representations in the Human Neocortex , 2019, Neuron.

[28]  Elger L. Abrahamse,et al.  Cognitive Processing in New and Practiced Discrete Keying Sequences , 2010, Front. Psychology.

[29]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  Task-set reconfiguration with predictable and unpredictable task switches , 2003, Memory & cognition.

[30]  Gordon D. Logan,et al.  Executive Control of Thought and Action , 2003 .

[31]  S. Keele,et al.  The cognitive and neural architecture of sequence representation. , 2003, Psychological review.

[32]  J. Doyon,et al.  Reorganization and plasticity in the adult brain during learning of motor skills , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[33]  Massimo Riani,et al.  An fMRI study of the role of noise in image processing by the human perceptive system , 2001, NeuroImage.

[34]  A. Reber,et al.  Another wrinkle on the dual-task SRT experiment: It’s probably not dual task , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[35]  Beat Meier,et al.  Correlation and response relevance in sequence learning , 2012, Psychological Research.

[36]  Michael Matessa,et al.  A Production System Theory of Serial Memory , 1997 .

[37]  Gordon D Logan,et al.  Automatic Control: How Experts Act Without Thinking , 2018, Psychological review.

[38]  Axel Cleeremans,et al.  The neural correlates of implicit and explicit sequence learning: Interacting networks revealed by the process dissociation procedure. , 2005, Learning & memory.

[39]  Tom Manly,et al.  We do as we construe: extended behavior construed as one task is executed as one cognitive entity , 2018, Psychological Research.

[40]  A. Gordon,et al.  Choosing between movement sequences: A hierarchical editor model. , 1984 .

[41]  Darryl W. Schneider,et al.  Hierarchical control of cognitive processes: switching tasks in sequences. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[42]  Saul Sternberg,et al.  Hierarchical Control in the Execution of Action Sequences: Tests of Two Invariance Properties , 2018, Attention and Performance XIII.

[43]  G. D. Logan Task Switching , 2022 .

[44]  Eva Röttger,et al.  Sequence Knowledge on When and What Supports Dual-Tasking , 2019, Journal of cognition.

[45]  Theresa M. Desrochers,et al.  The Necessity of Rostrolateral Prefrontal Cortex for Higher-Level Sequential Behavior , 2015, Neuron.

[46]  Tim Curran,et al.  Attentional and Nonattentional Forms of Sequence Learning , 1993 .

[47]  L. Cohen,et al.  Neuroplasticity Subserving Motor Skill Learning , 2011, Neuron.

[48]  E. Robertson The Serial Reaction Time Task: Implicit Motor Skill Learning? , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  H. Hughes,et al.  On the minimization of task switch costs following long-term training , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[50]  John Duncan,et al.  Hierarchical Organization of Cognition Reflected in Distributed Frontoparietal Activity , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  L. Snyder,et al.  Extensive practice does not eliminate human switch costs , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.