Agreement Between Scales in the Measurement of Breast Cancer Risk Perceptions

The objective of this article is to compare the accuracy and numeric responses of breast cancer risk perception as measured by a frequency scale and percentage scale. A cross-sectional survey was conducted. Perceptions of five-year and lifetime breast cancer risk were measured using a frequency and a percentage scale. Estimation error was calculated as the absolute difference between actual breast cancer risk as determined by the Gail model and perceived risk. Agreement between scales was determined by calculating the mean and standard deviation of the difference between numeric responses. The study was conducted among women enrolled in two primary care clinics associated with an academic medical center. Two-hundred-fifty-four participants were recruited from one of the two participating internal medicine clinics. Inclusion criteria included female gender and age 40-84 years. Exclusion criteria included a history of breast cancer, dementia, or a life expectancy of less than two years. The frequency scale was more accurate than the percentage scale in estimating lifetime risk (p= 0.05), but less accurate in estimating five-year risk (p < 0.02). Only 79 participants (31%) were considered consistent scale users, providing identical responses when using the frequency and percentage scale for a given risk estimate. Although the mean difference (percentage-frequency scale) for estimates of breast cancer lifetime risk was only 2.4, the empirically determined 90% limits of agreement between the frequency and percentage scale for lifetime risk were wide, from -30 to 40. Higher numeracy was associated with consistent use of scales (OR 1.61, 95% CI; 1.09-2.37). We report disagreement in breast cancer risk perceptions when measured by a frequency and a percentage scale. The accuracy and direction of bias associated with each scale varies according to the time frame of risk being assessed.

[1]  Kimihiko Yamagishi When a 12.86% Mortality is More Dangerous than 24.14%: Implications for Risk Communication , 1997 .

[2]  Gerd Gigerenzer,et al.  Communicating Statistical Information , 2000, Science.

[3]  D H Hickam,et al.  Patients’ interpretations of probability terms , 1991, Journal of general internal medicine.

[4]  P. Slovic,et al.  Violence Risk Assessment and Risk Communication: The Effects of Using Actual Cases, Providing Instruction, and Employing Probability Versus Frequency Formats , 2000, Law and human behavior.

[5]  S. Pepper,et al.  Sometimes frequently means seldom: Context effects in the interpretation of quantitative expressions ☆ , 1974 .

[6]  B Fischhoff,et al.  A New Scale for Assessing Perceptions of Chance , 2000, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[7]  D A Redelmeier,et al.  Understanding patients' decisions. Cognitive and emotional perspectives. , 1993, JAMA.

[8]  G O Barnett,et al.  How medical professionals evaluate expressions of probability. , 1986, The New England journal of medicine.

[9]  M. Ruffin,et al.  Patients' interpretation of qualitative probability statements. , 1994, Archives of family medicine.

[10]  E. J. Mayeaux,et al.  Rapid estimate of adult literacy in medicine: a shortened screening instrument. , 1993, Family medicine.

[11]  I D Graham,et al.  Decision aids for patients considering options affecting cancer outcomes: evidence of efficacy and policy implications. , 1999, Journal of the National Cancer Institute. Monographs.

[12]  Taylor Murray,et al.  Cancer Statistics, 2001 , 2001, CA: a cancer journal for clinicians.

[13]  D Spiegelman,et al.  Validation of the Gail et al. model of breast cancer risk prediction and implications for chemoprevention. , 2001, Journal of the National Cancer Institute.

[14]  H. Welch,et al.  Women's Perceptions of Breast Cancer Risk , 1999, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[15]  D. Altman,et al.  STATISTICAL METHODS FOR ASSESSING AGREEMENT BETWEEN TWO METHODS OF CLINICAL MEASUREMENT , 1986, The Lancet.

[16]  Neil D. Weinstein,et al.  Testing four competing theories of health-protective behavior. , 1993 .

[17]  D. Grimes,et al.  Patients' understanding of medical risks: implications for genetic counseling. , 1999, Obstetrics and gynecology.

[18]  A. Howell,et al.  Perception of risk in women with a family history of breast cancer. , 1993, British Journal of Cancer.

[19]  M. Banaji,et al.  Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. , 1995, Psychological review.

[20]  D. Lane,et al.  Risk perception, family history, and use of breast cancer screening tests. , 1991, Cancer detection and prevention.

[21]  G Gigerenzer,et al.  Using natural frequencies to improve diagnostic inferences , 1998, Academic medicine : journal of the Association of American Medical Colleges.

[22]  J Benichou,et al.  Validation studies for models projecting the risk of invasive and total breast cancer incidence. , 1999, Journal of the National Cancer Institute.

[23]  A. Tversky,et al.  Choices, Values, and Frames , 2000 .

[24]  Kimihiko Yamagishi Upward versus downward anchoring in frequency judgments of social facts , 1997 .

[25]  R F Nease,et al.  Perceptions of breast cancer risk and screening effectiveness in women younger than 50 years of age. , 1995, Journal of the National Cancer Institute.

[26]  B. Rimer,et al.  Relationships among objective and subjective risk for breast cancer and mammography stages of change. , 1996, Cancer epidemiology, biomarkers & prevention : a publication of the American Association for Cancer Research, cosponsored by the American Society of Preventive Oncology.

[27]  M. Nakao,et al.  Numbers are better than words. Verbal specifications of frequency have no place in medicine. , 1983, The American journal of medicine.

[28]  B. Rimer,et al.  Effects of individualized breast cancer risk counseling: a randomized trial. , 1995, Journal of the National Cancer Institute.

[29]  A. Beckett,et al.  AKUFO AND IBARAPA. , 1965, Lancet.

[30]  C. McHorney,et al.  Frequency or Probability? A Qualitative Study of Risk Communication Formats Used in Health Care , 2001, Medical decision making : an international journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making.

[31]  Lisa M. Schwartz,et al.  The Role of Numeracy in Understanding the Benefit of Screening Mammography , 1997, Annals of Internal Medicine.