Exploring the consequences of the previous trial

In tasks that are designed to explore cognitive functioning, the response on each trial is a function of the combination of experimental conditions that occurred on that and the previous trial. Because the previous trial influences performance, the event presented during or the action required by the previous trial must leave an imprint on the brain's activity that carries through to the next trial. These imprints are manifest in the activity of single neurons that participate in producing the response. Previous trial effects address disparate cognitive phenomena, such as response priming, task switching and inhibition of return, and the neural bases of previous trial effects can be envisioned as changes in salience of the target or the goal of the action on a spatial map.

[1]  J. M. Cattell THE TIME TAKEN UP BY CEREBRAL OPERATIONS , 1886 .

[2]  P. Bertelson Sequential Redundancy and Speed in a Serial Two-Choice Responding Task , 1961 .

[3]  M I Posner,et al.  Chronometric analysis of classification. , 1967, Psychological review.

[4]  R. Remington Analysis of sequential effects in choice reaction times. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.

[5]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Activity of superior colliculus in behaving monkey. I. Visual receptive fields of single neurons. , 1972, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Activity of superior colliculus in behaving monkey. 3. Cells discharging before eye movements. , 1972, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  N. Kirby Sequential effects in two-choice reaction time: automatic facilitation or subjective expectancy? , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  D. Sparks Functional properties of neurons in the monkey superior colliculus: Coupling of neuronal activity and saccade onset , 1978, Brain Research.

[9]  W. Becker,et al.  An analysis of the saccadic system by means of double step stimuli , 1979, Vision Research.

[10]  S Ullman,et al.  Shifts in selective visual attention: towards the underlying neural circuitry. , 1985, Human neurobiology.

[11]  D. Schacter,et al.  Implicit and explicit memory for new associations in normal and amnesic subjects. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[12]  E. Soetens,et al.  Expectancy or Automatic Facilitation? Separating Sequential Effects in Two-Choice Reaction Time , 1985 .

[13]  M. Posner,et al.  Inhibition of return : Neural basis and function , 1985 .

[14]  S. Tipper The Negative Priming Effect: Inhibitory Priming by Ignored Objects , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[15]  R. Duncan Luce,et al.  Response Times: Their Role in Inferring Elementary Mental Organization , 1986 .

[16]  M. W. Brown,et al.  Neuronal evidence that inferomedial temporal cortex is more important than hippocampus in certain processes underlying recognition memory , 1987, Brain Research.

[17]  P. C. Murphy,et al.  Cerebral Cortex , 2017, Cerebral Cortex.

[18]  Raymond Klein,et al.  Inhibitory tagging system facilitates visual search , 1988, Nature.

[19]  Allen Allport,et al.  Visual attention , 1989 .

[20]  P. Calabresi,et al.  Saccade preparation inhibits reorienting to recently attended locations. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  Peter Bullemer,et al.  On the development of procedural knowledge. , 1989 .

[22]  R. Wurtz,et al.  The Neurobiology of Saccadic Eye Movements , 1989 .

[23]  E Tulving,et al.  Priming and human memory systems. , 1990, Science.

[24]  L E Mays,et al.  Signal transformations required for the generation of saccadic eye movements. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[25]  D. Pélisson,et al.  Control of orienting gaze shifts by the tectoreticulospinal system in the head-free cat. III. Spatiotemporal characteristics of phasic motor discharges. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  R. Desimone,et al.  A neural mechanism for working and recognition memory in inferior temporal cortex. , 1991, Science.

[27]  M. Bravo,et al.  The role of attention in different visual-search tasks , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[28]  S. Petersen,et al.  The pulvinar and visual salience , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[29]  David L. Sparks,et al.  Movement selection in advance of action in the superior colliculus , 1992, Nature.

[30]  Jeffrey D. Schall,et al.  Neural basis of saccade target selection in frontal eye field during visual search , 1993, Nature.

[31]  S. Tipper,et al.  Object-based and environment-based inhibition of return of visual attention. , 1994 .

[32]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Priming of pop-out: I. Role of features , 1994, Memory & cognition.

[33]  David L. Sparks,et al.  Systematic errors for saccades to remembered targets: Evidence for a dissociation between saccade metrics and activity in the superior colliculus , 1994, Vision Research.

[34]  S. Tipper,et al.  Object-based and environment-based inhibition of return of visual attention. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[35]  J. Schall,et al.  Saccade target selection in frontal eye field of macaque. I. Visual and premovement activation , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[36]  D L Robinson,et al.  Covert orienting of attention in macaques. III. Contributions of the superior colliculus. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  Cynthia P. May,et al.  Determinants of negative priming. , 1995, Psychological bulletin.

[38]  S. Monsell,et al.  Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .

[39]  D. Robinson,et al.  Covert orienting of attention in macaques. II. Contributions of parietal cortex. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Saccade-related activity in monkey superior colliculus. I. Characteristics of burst and buildup cells. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Priming of pop-out: II. The role of position , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.

[42]  J. Schall,et al.  Neural Control of Voluntary Movement Initiation , 1996, Science.

[43]  D. Robinson,et al.  Shared neural control of attentional shifts and eye movements , 1996, Nature.

[44]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Priming of popout: II. Role of position , 1996 .

[45]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms for visual memory and their role in attention. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[46]  S. Shimojo,et al.  Location vs Feature: Reaction Time Reveals Dissociation Between Two Visual Functions , 1996, Vision Research.

[47]  C. Scudder,et al.  The microscopic anatomy and physiology of the mammalian saccadic system , 1996, Progress in Neurobiology.

[48]  L. Robertson,et al.  Attentional persistence for features of hierarchical patterns. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[49]  Michele A. Basso,et al.  Modulation of neuronal activity by target uncertainty , 1997, Nature.

[50]  J. Pratt,et al.  Inhibition of return in location- and identity-based choice decision tasks , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[51]  A. Dale,et al.  Functional-Anatomic Correlates of Object Priming in Humans Revealed by Rapid Presentation Event-Related fMRI , 1998, Neuron.

[52]  M. A. Basso,et al.  Modulation of Neuronal Activity in Superior Colliculus by Changes in Target Probability , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[53]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Reversible inactivation of monkey superior colliculus. I. Curvature of saccadic trajectory. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[54]  M. Goldberg,et al.  The representation of visual salience in monkey parietal cortex , 1998, Nature.

[55]  G. Caputo,et al.  Attentional selection by distractor suppression , 1998, Vision Research.

[56]  D. E. Irwin,et al.  Our Eyes do Not Always Go Where we Want Them to Go: Capture of the Eyes by New Objects , 1998 .

[57]  E. Soetens Localizing sequential effects in serial choice reaction time with the information reduction procedure , 1998 .

[58]  Raymond M. Klein,et al.  On the causes and effects of inhibition of return , 1998 .

[59]  S. Tipper,et al.  Scene-based and object-centered inhibition of return: Evidence for dual orienting mechanisms , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[60]  D P Munoz,et al.  Influence of previous visual stimulus or saccade on saccadic reaction times in monkey. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[61]  R. Ivry,et al.  Sequential priming in hierarchically organized figures: effects of target level and target resolution. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[62]  M Behrmann,et al.  Attention accesses multiple reference frames: evidence from visual neglect. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[63]  Robert M. McPeek,et al.  Saccades require focal attention and are facilitated by a short-term memory system , 1999, Vision Research.

[64]  J. Schall,et al.  Neural selection and control of visually guided eye movements. , 1999, Annual review of neuroscience.

[65]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Effects of similarity and history on neural mechanisms of visual selection , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[66]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Space and attention in parietal cortex. , 1999, Annual review of neuroscience.

[67]  David E. Irwin,et al.  Influence of attentional capture on oculomotor control. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[68]  Nick Fogt,et al.  The Neurology of Eye Movements, 3rd ed. , 2000 .

[69]  R. Klein,et al.  Visual and motor effects in inhibition of return. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[70]  D. Munoz,et al.  t Immediate Neural Plasticity Shapes Motor Performance , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[71]  A. Cowey,et al.  Normal discrimination performance accompanied by priming deficits in monkeys with V4 or TEO lesions. , 2000, Neuroreport.

[72]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Role of the basal ganglia in the control of purposive saccadic eye movements. , 2000, Physiological reviews.

[73]  D P Munoz,et al.  On your mark, get set: brainstem circuitry underlying saccadic initiation. , 2000, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[74]  Repetition priming reveals sustained facilitation and transient inhibition in reaction time. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[75]  A. Sereno,et al.  Inhibition of return in manual and saccadic response systems , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[76]  R. Klein,et al.  On the manifestations of memory in visual search. , 2000, Spatial vision.

[77]  C. Koch,et al.  A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention , 2000, Vision Research.

[78]  J. Pratt,et al.  Oculocentric coding of inhibited eye movements to recently attended locations. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[79]  Robert M. McPeek,et al.  Concurrent processing of saccades in visual search , 2000, Vision Research.

[80]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Priming of popout: III. A short-term implicit memory system beneficial for rapid target selection , 2000 .

[81]  R. Klein,et al.  Inhibition of return , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[82]  N. Shimizu [Neurology of eye movements]. , 2000, Rinsho shinkeigaku = Clinical neurology.

[83]  T. Shallice,et al.  Neuroimaging evidence for dissociable forms of repetition priming. , 2000, Science.

[84]  J. Driver,et al.  Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII , 2000 .

[85]  Joel L. Davis,et al.  Visual attention and cortical circuits , 2001 .

[86]  C. Koch,et al.  Computational modelling of visual attention , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[87]  S. Tipper,et al.  On the Strategic Modulation of the Time Course of Facilitation and Inhibition of Return , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[88]  W T Newsome,et al.  Target selection for saccadic eye movements: prelude activity in the superior colliculus during a direction-discrimination task. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[89]  R. Walker,et al.  Curved saccade trajectories: Voluntary and reflexive saccades curve away from irrelevant distractors , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[90]  S. Tipper Does Negative Priming Reflect Inhibitory Mechanisms? A Review and Integration of Conflicting Views , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[91]  Takashi R Sato,et al.  Search Efficiency but Not Response Interference Affects Visual Selection in Frontal Eye Field , 2001, Neuron.

[92]  E. Keller,et al.  Short-term priming, concurrent processing, and saccade curvature during a target selection task in the monkey , 2001, Vision Research.

[93]  J D Schall,et al.  Dynamic dissociation of visual selection from saccade programming in frontal eye field. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[94]  N. Cohen,et al.  Theoretical and computational analysis of skill learning, repetition priming, and procedural memory. , 2002, Psychological review.

[95]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Visual and Anticipatory Bias in Three Cortical Eye Fields of the Monkey during an Adaptive Decision-Making Task , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[96]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Priming in Macaque Frontal Cortex during Popout Visual Search: Feature-Based Facilitation and Location-Based Inhibition of Return , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[97]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Oculomotor capture and Inhibition of Return: Evidence for an oculomotor suppression account of IOR , 2002, Psychological research.

[98]  D. Sparks The brainstem control of saccadic eye movements , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[99]  D P Munoz,et al.  Time course of a repetition effect on saccadic reaction time in non-human primates. , 2002, Archives italiennes de biologie.

[100]  R. Klein,et al.  Contribution of the Primate Superior Colliculus to Inhibition of Return , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[101]  T. Shallice,et al.  Task Switching : A PDP Model , 2001 .

[102]  M. Donnelly,et al.  Inhibition of return for target discriminations: The effect of repeating discriminated and irrelevant stimulus dimensions , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.

[103]  Robert M. McPeek,et al.  Superior colliculus activity related to concurrent processing of saccade goals in a visual search task. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[104]  Robert M McPeek,et al.  Neural Discharge in the Superior Colliculus during Target Search Paradigms , 2002, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[105]  A. Fuchs,et al.  The brainstem burst generator for saccadic eye movements , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[106]  E. Keller,et al.  Saccade target selection in the superior colliculus during a visual search task. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[107]  R. Henson,et al.  Neural response suppression, haemodynamic repetition effects, and behavioural priming , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[108]  Robert M McPeek,et al.  Competition between saccade goals in the superior colliculus produces saccade curvature. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[109]  E. Rolls,et al.  Responses of neurons in the inferior temporal cortex in short term and serial recognition memory tasks , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[110]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Orienting of attention and eye movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.