Motivation alters response bias and neural activation patterns in a perceptual decision-making task

Motivation has been demonstrated to affect individuals' response strategies in economic decision-making, however, little is known about how motivation influences perceptual decision-making behavior or its related neural activity. Given the important role motivation plays in shaping our behavior, a better understanding of this relationship is needed. A block-design, continuous performance, perceptual decision-making task where participants were asked to detect a picture of an animal among distractors was used during functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). The effect of positive and negative motivation on sustained activity within regions of the brain thought to underlie decision-making was examined by altering the monetary contingency associated with the task. In addition, signal detection theory was used to investigate the effect of motivation on detection sensitivity, response bias and response time. While both positive and negative motivation resulted in increased sustained activation in the ventral striatum, fusiform gyrus, left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and ventromedial prefrontal cortex, only negative motivation resulted in the adoption of a more liberal, closer to optimal response bias. This shift toward a liberal response bias correlated with increased activation in the left DLPFC, but did not result in improved task performance. The present findings suggest that motivation alters aspects of the way perceptual decisions are made. Further, this altered response behavior is reflected in a change in left DLPFC activation, a region involved in the computation of perceptual decisions.

[1]  S. Huettel,et al.  Human Neuroscience , 2022 .

[2]  Matthew T. Kaufman,et al.  Distributed Neural Representation of Expected Value , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  Jeffrey B. Henriques,et al.  Reward fails to alter response bias in depression. , 1994, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[4]  Henrik Walter,et al.  Prediction error as a linear function of reward probability is coded in human nucleus accumbens , 2006, NeuroImage.

[5]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Neural Coding of Tactile Decisions in the Human Prefrontal Cortex , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  C. Carver,et al.  Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales , 1994 .

[7]  N. Macmillan,et al.  Response bias : characteristics of detection theory, threshold theory, and nonparametric indexes , 1990 .

[8]  R. Elliott,et al.  Dissociable functions in the medial and lateral orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from human neuroimaging studies. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[9]  M. Sahani,et al.  Effects of Category-Specific Costs on Neural Systems for Perceptual Decision-Making , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  J. W. Aldridge,et al.  Dissecting components of reward: 'liking', 'wanting', and learning. , 2009, Current opinion in pharmacology.

[11]  L. Pessoa,et al.  Combined Effects of Attention and Motivation on Visual Task Performance: Transient and Sustained Motivational Effects , 2008, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[12]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[13]  M. Delgado,et al.  Reward‐Related Responses in the Human Striatum , 2007, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[14]  F. Blankenburg,et al.  Causal Role of Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex in Human Perceptual Decision Making , 2011, Current Biology.

[15]  L. Pessoa,et al.  Motivation sharpens exogenous spatial attention. , 2007, Emotion.

[16]  Hakwan Lau,et al.  Prior Expectation Modulates the Interaction between Sensory and Prefrontal Regions in the Human Brain , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Neural correlates of a decision in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the macaque , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[18]  N. Volkow,et al.  “Nonhedonic” food motivation in humans involves dopamine in the dorsal striatum and methylphenidate amplifies this effect , 2002, Synapse.

[19]  Irwin P. Levin,et al.  Risk preferences in young children: early evidence of individual differences in reaction to potential gains and losses , 2003 .

[20]  R. Elliott,et al.  Medial orbitofrontal cortex codes relative rather than absolute value of financial rewards in humans , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.

[21]  A. Tversky,et al.  The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. , 1981, Science.

[22]  N. Mashal,et al.  Right hemisphere sensitivity to novel metaphoric relations: Application of the signal detection theory , 2008, Brain and Language.

[23]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[24]  M. Farah,et al.  Ventromedial frontal cortex mediates affective shifting in humans: evidence from a reversal learning paradigm. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[25]  Samuel M. McClure,et al.  Temporal Prediction Errors in a Passive Learning Task Activate Human Striatum , 2003, Neuron.

[26]  Sabrina M. Tom,et al.  The Neural Basis of Loss Aversion in Decision-Making Under Risk , 2007, Science.

[27]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect Theory : An Analysis of Decision under Risk Author ( s ) : , 2007 .

[28]  Yul-Wan Sung,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging , 2004, Scholarpedia.

[29]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Sensory-motor mechanisms in human parietal cortex underlie arbitrary visual decisions , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[30]  Neil A. Macmillan,et al.  Detection Theory: A User's Guide , 1991 .

[31]  C. Summerfield,et al.  Economic Value Biases Uncertain Perceptual Choices in the Parietal and Prefrontal Cortices , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[32]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Bias in the Brain: A Diffusion Model Analysis of Prior Probability and Potential Payoff , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  Craig E. L. Stark,et al.  When zero is not zero: The problem of ambiguous baseline conditions in fMRI , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[34]  K. Luan Phan,et al.  A functional neuroimaging study of motivation and executive function , 2004, NeuroImage.

[35]  June Corwin,et al.  On measuring discrimination and response bias: Unequal numbers of targets and distractors and two classes of distractors. , 1994 .

[36]  M. Farah,et al.  Different underlying impairments in decision-making following ventromedial and dorsolateral frontal lobe damage in humans. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[37]  David A. Medler,et al.  Neural correlates of sensory and decision processes in auditory object identification , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[38]  J. Salamone The involvement of nucleus accumbens dopamine in appetitive and aversive motivation , 1994, Behavioural Brain Research.

[39]  J L Lancaster,et al.  Neuroscience on the net. , 1994, Science.

[40]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  A general mechanism for perceptual decision-making in the human brain , 2004, Nature.

[41]  J. A. Frost,et al.  Conceptual Processing during the Conscious Resting State: A Functional MRI Study , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[42]  L. Shah,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 2010, Seminars in roentgenology.

[43]  D. M. Green,et al.  Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .

[44]  Corey J. Bohil,et al.  Category discriminability, base-rate, and payoff effects in perceptual categorization , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[45]  J. Gray,et al.  The psychophysiological basis of introversion-extraversion. , 1970, Behaviour research and therapy.

[46]  Peter Dayan,et al.  A Neural Substrate of Prediction and Reward , 1997, Science.

[47]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Involvement of human left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in perceptual decision making is independent of response modality , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[48]  A. Voss,et al.  Perception of danger signals: the role of control. , 2004, Experimental psychology.

[49]  H. Damasio,et al.  Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[50]  M. F. Luce,et al.  Separate Neural Mechanisms Underlie Choices and Strategic Preferences in Risky Decision Making , 2009, Neuron.

[51]  M. Steyvers,et al.  The dynamics of experimentally induced criterion shifts. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[52]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Encoding Predictive Reward Value in Human Amygdala and Orbitofrontal Cortex , 2003, Science.

[53]  G. Glover,et al.  The Role of Ventral Frontostriatal Circuitry in Reward-Based Learning in Humans , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[54]  Brian Knutson,et al.  Anticipation of Increasing Monetary Reward Selectively Recruits Nucleus Accumbens , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[55]  S. Ikemoto,et al.  The role of nucleus accumbens dopamine in motivated behavior: a unifying interpretation with special reference to reward-seeking , 1999, Brain Research Reviews.

[56]  Lars Kai Hansen,et al.  Finding related functional neuroimaging volumes , 2004, Artif. Intell. Medicine.

[57]  Brian Knutson,et al.  Valence and salience contribute to nucleus accumbens activation , 2008, NeuroImage.

[58]  P. Dayan,et al.  Behavioral/systems/cognitive Action Dominates Valence in Anticipatory Representations in the Human Striatum and Dopaminergic Midbrain , 2010 .

[59]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation-Induced Changes in Sensorimotor Coupling Parallel Improvements of Somatosensation in Humans , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.