Implicit prototypes predict risky sun behavior.

OBJECTIVE Despite the fact that skin cancer is highly avoidable, incidence and death rates in the United States continue to climb. The pattern is particularly problematic among young, White women, who sometimes overexpose themselves to harmful ultraviolet rays in hopes of being tan. Research has suggested that positivity toward prototypes of individuals who engage in unhealthy behavior, like tanning, influences the likelihood that an individual will personally engage in those behaviors. Although the prototype-to-behavior link is considered to operate automatically, researchers have typically relied on people's self-reported evaluations of prototypes, which are more controlled and susceptible to self-presentational concerns. METHOD In the present research, we developed a measure of implicit prototypes and compared it with measures of explicit prototypes in predicting the safe sun behavior of 731 women. RESULTS Meta-analysis of 5 different prototypes (i.e., cool, fun, healthy, intelligent, and attractive) suggested that implicit prototypes predicted more variance in women's current behavior, planned behavior, behavioral willingness, and tanning frequency than did explicit prototypes. CONCLUSION Although some models recognize that health behavior may be based on automatic processes, they exclusively use measures of self-reported attitudes and prototypes to predict behavior. The results suggest that measuring implicit prototypes may provide important explanatory power.

[1]  J. Kulik,et al.  Effects of Two Appearance-Based Interventions on the Sun Protection Behaviors of Southern California Beach Patrons , 2006 .

[2]  P. Devine,et al.  Stereotyping and evaluation in implicit race bias: evidence for independent constructs and unique effects on behavior. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[3]  A. Rademaker,et al.  Summer sun exposure: knowledge, attitudes, and behaviors of Midwest adolescents. , 1997, Preventive medicine.

[4]  Michael A. Olson,et al.  Reducing Automatically Activated Racial Prejudice Through Implicit Evaluative Conditioning , 2006, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[5]  G. Saad,et al.  Applying Darwinian principles in designing effective intervention strategies: The case of sun tanning , 2006 .

[6]  D. Vogel,et al.  Assessing the utility of the willingness/prototype model in predicting help-seeking decisions. , 2013, Journal of counseling psychology.

[7]  J. Hillhouse,et al.  Predictors of sunbathing and sunscreen use in college undergraduates , 1996, Journal of Behavioral Medicine.

[8]  J. Dovidio,et al.  Distance Makes the Heart Grow Fonder : Improving Implicit Racial Attitudes and Interracial Interactions Through Approach Behaviors , 2007 .

[9]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  Evaluative Conditioning and Conscious Knowledge of Contingencies: A Correlational Investigation with Large Samples , 2010, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[10]  D. Packer,et al.  Increasing Implicit Self-Esteem Through Classical Conditioning , 2004, Psychological science.

[11]  M. Banaji,et al.  Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. , 1995, Psychological review.

[12]  R. Vogel,et al.  Time to Get Serious About Skin Cancer Prevention , 2012, Cancer Epidemiology, Biomarkers & Prevention.

[13]  F. Gibbons,et al.  Images of smokers and willingness to smoke among African American pre-adolescents: an application of the prototype/willingness model of adolescent health risk behavior to smoking initiation. , 2005, Journal of pediatric psychology.

[14]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  Understanding and Using the Implicit Association Test: II. Method Variables and Construct Validity , 2005, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[15]  A. Lenton,et al.  Imagining stereotypes away: the moderation of implicit stereotypes through mental imagery. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[16]  A. Greenwald,et al.  Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[17]  K. Glanz,et al.  Preventing skin cancer: findings of the Task Force on Community Preventive Services On reducing Exposure to Ultraviolet Light. , 2003, MMWR. Recommendations and reports : Morbidity and mortality weekly report. Recommendations and reports.

[18]  M. Banaji,et al.  PREDICTIVE VALIDITY OF THE IAT 1 RUNNING HEAD : PREDICTIVE VALIDITY OF THE IAT Understanding and Using the Implicit Association Test : III . Meta-analysis of Predictive Validity , 2006 .

[19]  Kumar Yogeeswaran,et al.  Will the “Real” American Please Stand Up? The Effect of Implicit National Prototypes on Discriminatory Behavior and Judgments , 2010, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[20]  F. Gibbons,et al.  A dual-process approach to health risk decision making: The prototype willingness model , 2008 .

[21]  Frederick X. Gibbons,et al.  A Social Reaction Model of Adolescent Health Risk , 2009 .

[22]  Christopher J Armitage,et al.  Augmenting the theory of planned behaviour with the prototype/willingness model: predictive validity of actor versus abstainer prototypes for adolescents' health-protective and health-risk intentions. , 2006, British journal of health psychology.

[23]  Jakob Heydenreich,et al.  UV radiation exposure related to age, sex, occupation, and sun behavior based on time-stamped personal dosimeter readings. , 2004, Archives of dermatology.

[24]  Skyler T. Hawk,et al.  Presentation and validation of the Radboud Faces Database , 2010 .

[25]  J. Leonardi-Bee,et al.  A systematic review of worldwide incidence of nonmelanoma skin cancer , 2012, The British journal of dermatology.

[26]  L. Naldi,et al.  The epidemiology of skin cancer , 2002, The British journal of dermatology.

[27]  Pragna Patel,et al.  Recent trends in cutaneous melanoma incidence and death rates in the United States, 1992-2006. , 2011, Journal of the American Academy of Dermatology.

[28]  Lawrence E Gibson,et al.  Increasing incidence of melanoma among young adults: an epidemiological study in Olmsted County, Minnesota. , 2012, Mayo Clinic proceedings.

[29]  M. van Ryn,et al.  The effect of patient race and socio-economic status on physicians' perceptions of patients. , 2000, Social science & medicine.

[30]  B. Lloyd,et al.  Smoking in Adolescence: Images and Identities , 1998 .

[31]  Scott B. MacKenzie,et al.  Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. , 2003, The Journal of applied psychology.

[32]  C. Judd,et al.  The police officer's dilemma: using ethnicity to disambiguate potentially threatening individuals. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[33]  R. Engels,et al.  Explaining adolescents' smoking and drinking behavior: the concept of smoker and drinker prototypes in relation to variables of the theory of planned behavior. , 2004, Addictive behaviors.

[34]  Gail M. Williams,et al.  Prolonged Prevention of Squamous Cell Carcinoma of the Skin by Regular Sunscreen Use , 2006, Cancer Epidemiology Biomarkers & Prevention.

[35]  M. Leary,et al.  The Social Psychology of Tanning and Sunscreen Use: Self‐Presentational Motives as a Predictor of Health Risk1 , 1993 .

[36]  K. Witte Putting the fear back into fear appeals: The extended parallel process model , 1992 .

[37]  R. Wiers,et al.  Retraining Automatic Action Tendencies Changes Alcoholic Patients’ Approach Bias for Alcohol and Improves Treatment Outcome , 2011, Psychological science.

[38]  I. Ajzen The theory of planned behavior , 1991 .

[39]  J. Kulik,et al.  Using UV photography to reduce use of tanning booths: a test of cognitive mediation. , 2005, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[40]  E. Rosch ON THE INTERNAL STRUCTURE OF PERCEPTUAL AND SEMANTIC CATEGORIES1 , 1973 .

[41]  F. Gibbons,et al.  Reason and reaction: the utility of a dual-focus, dual-processing perspective on promotion and prevention of adolescent health risk behaviour. , 2009, British journal of health psychology.

[42]  I. Ajzen The theory of planned behaviour: Reactions and reflections , 2011, Psychology & health.

[43]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  Reducing Implicit Prejudice , 2013 .

[44]  Jeffrey C. Valentine,et al.  Research Synthesis and Meta-Analysis , 2008 .

[45]  Silje S. Hukkelberg,et al.  Using the Prototype/Willingness model to predict smoking behaviour among Norwegian adolescents. , 2009, Addictive behaviors.

[46]  C. Judd,et al.  Spontaneous prejudice in context: variability in automatically activated attitudes. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[47]  Elliot T. Berkman,et al.  Neural activity during health messaging predicts reductions in smoking above and beyond self-report. , 2011, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[48]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  Contextual variations in implicit evaluation. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[49]  B. Mullan,et al.  Using the theory of planned behaviour and prototype willingness model to target binge drinking in female undergraduate university students. , 2011, Addictive behaviors.

[50]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  Understanding and using the implicit association test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. , 2003, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[51]  Cassandra L. Govan,et al.  Changing the affective valence of the stimulus items influences the IAT by re-defining the category labels , 2004 .

[52]  June K. Robinson,et al.  Indoor tanning knowledge, attitudes, and behavior among young adults from 1988-2007. , 2008, Archives of dermatology.

[53]  L. Aiken,et al.  A psychosocial model of sun protection and sunbathing in young women: the impact of health beliefs, attitudes, norms, and self-efficacy for sun protection. , 2000, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[54]  Joanne H. Gerrits,et al.  Cool and independent or foolish and undisciplined? Adolescents’ prototypes of (un)healthy eaters and their association with eating behaviour , 2009, Appetite.

[55]  S. Stark,et al.  An investigation of appearance motives for tanning: The development and evaluation of the Physical Appearance Reasons For Tanning Scale (PARTS) and its relation to sunbathing and indoor tanning intentions. , 2006, Body image.

[56]  P. Fletcher,et al.  Changing Human Behavior to Prevent Disease: The Importance of Targeting Automatic Processes , 2012, Science.

[57]  M. Conner,et al.  Prospective prediction of health-related behaviours with the Theory of Planned Behaviour: a meta-analysis , 2011 .

[58]  Implicit Science Stereotypes Mediate the Relationship between Gender and Academic Participation , 2012 .

[59]  M. Becker,et al.  The Health Belief Model: A Decade Later , 1984, Health education quarterly.

[60]  E. Farmer,et al.  Awareness of the risks of tanning lamps does not influence behavior among college students. , 2002, Archives of dermatology.

[61]  Emily B. Falk,et al.  Predicting Persuasion-Induced Behavior Change from the Brain , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[62]  F. Gibbons,et al.  Predicting young adults' health risk behavior. , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[63]  Jan De Houwer,et al.  What Are Implicit Measures and Why Are We Using Them , 2006 .

[64]  D. Hermans,et al.  On the acquisition and activation of evaluative information in memory: The study of evaluative learning and affective priming combined , 2003 .

[65]  Bertram Gawronski,et al.  The Associative–Propositional Evaluation Model: Theory, Evidence, and Open Questions , 2011 .

[66]  R. Turrisi,et al.  Motivations for Indoor Tanning: Theoretical Models , 2012 .

[67]  H. Friedman,et al.  Psychosocial factors in sunbathing and sunscreen use. , 1987, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[68]  C. Abraham,et al.  Understanding young and older male drivers' willingness to drive while intoxicated: the predictive utility of constructs specified by the theory of planned behaviour and the prototype willingness model. , 2011, British journal of health psychology.

[69]  V. Hearing,et al.  UVA tanning is involved in the increased incidence of skin cancers in fair‐skinned young women , 2010, Pigment cell & melanoma research.

[70]  James A. Kulik,et al.  Effects of appearance-based interventions on sun protection intentions and self-reported behaviors. , 2003 .

[71]  J. Kulik,et al.  Long-term effects of appearance-based interventions on sun protection behaviors. , 2007, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[72]  E. Augustson,et al.  History and Culture of Tanning in the United States , 2012 .