Quick concurrent responses to global and local cognitive information underlie intuitive understanding in board-game experts

Experts have the superior cognitive capability of quickly understanding complex information in their domain; however, little is known about the neural processes underlying this ability. Here, using a board game named shogi (Japanese chess), we investigated the brain activity in expert players that was involved in their quick understanding of board-game patterns. The frontal area responded only to meaningful game positions, whereas the temporal area responded to both game and random positions with the same latency (200 ms). Subsequent to these quick responses, the temporal and parietal areas responded only to game positions, with a latency of 700 ms. During the responses, enhanced phase synchronization between these areas was observed. Thus, experts first responded to global cognitive information that was specific to game positions and to local cognitive information that was common to game and random positions concurrently. These types of information were integrated via neural synchronization at the posterior areas. As these properties were specific to experts, much of the experts' advantage in understanding game positions occurred within 1 s of perception.

[1]  W. Grodd,et al.  Many Faces of Expertise: Fusiform Face Area in Chess Experts and Novices , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  Herbert A. Simon,et al.  THE MIND'S EYE IN CHESS , 1988 .

[3]  J. Martinerie,et al.  Comparison of Hilbert transform and wavelet methods for the analysis of neuronal synchrony , 2001, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[4]  Daniel C. Krawczyk,et al.  Expertise and processing distorted structure in chess , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[5]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  The Neural Basis of Intuitive Best Next-Move Generation in Board Game Experts , 2011, Science.

[6]  J. Bullier Integrated model of visual processing , 2001, Brain Research Reviews.

[7]  Michael I. Posner,et al.  Event-Related Brain Potential Imaging of Semantic Encoding during Processing Single Words , 1998, NeuroImage.

[8]  M. Bar A Cortical Mechanism for Triggering Top-Down Facilitation in Visual Object Recognition , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[9]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Dissociation of object and spatial processing domains in primate prefrontal cortex. , 1993, Science.

[10]  E. Halgren,et al.  Top-down facilitation of visual recognition. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[11]  H. Simon,et al.  Recall of rapidly presented random chess positions is a function of skill , 1996, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[12]  William Bart,et al.  Moves in Mind: The Psychology of Board Games , 2012, Int. J. Gaming Comput. Mediat. Simulations.

[13]  A. L. Hosking,et al.  The Art of Shogi , 2003 .

[14]  F. Varela,et al.  Measuring phase synchrony in brain signals , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[15]  Erkki Oja,et al.  Independent component analysis: algorithms and applications , 2000, Neural Networks.

[16]  Michael Erb,et al.  Mechanisms and neural basis of object and pattern recognition: a study with chess experts. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[17]  H. Jasper Report of the committee on methods of clinical examination in electroencephalography , 1958 .

[18]  David J. Freedman,et al.  Categorical representation of visual stimuli in the primate prefrontal cortex. , 2001, Science.

[19]  Herbert A. Simon,et al.  Templates in Chess Memory: A Mechanism for Recalling Several Boards , 1996, Cognitive Psychology.

[20]  M. Eimer,et al.  An ERP study on the time course of emotional face processing , 2002, Neuroreport.

[21]  D. Hubel,et al.  Segregation of form, color, movement, and depth: anatomy, physiology, and perception. , 1988, Science.

[22]  J. Pine,et al.  Chunking mechanisms in human learning , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[23]  K. A. Ericsson,et al.  Expert and exceptional performance: evidence of maximal adaptation to task constraints. , 1996, Annual review of psychology.

[24]  Michael Erb,et al.  Expertise modulates the neural basis of context dependent recognition of objects and their relations , 2012, Human brain mapping.

[25]  Jia Liu,et al.  Perception of Face Parts and Face Configurations: An fMRI Study , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  Daniel C. Krawczyk,et al.  The neural organization of perception in chess experts , 2011, Neuroscience Letters.

[27]  A. D. D. Groot Thought and Choice in Chess , 1978 .

[28]  S. Petersen,et al.  Activation of extrastriate and frontal cortical areas by visual words and word-like stimuli. , 1990, Science.

[29]  H. Simon,et al.  Perception in chess , 1973 .

[30]  H. Simon,et al.  Recall of random and distorted chess positions: Implications for the theory of expertise , 1996, Memory & cognition.

[31]  M. Bar,et al.  Magnocellular Projections as the Trigger of Top-Down Facilitation in Recognition , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  Fernand Gobet,et al.  BRAIN LOCALIZATION OF MEMORY CHUNKS IN CHESSPLAYERS , 2007, The International journal of neuroscience.

[33]  D. Chakrabarti,et al.  A fast fixed - point algorithm for independent component analysis , 1997 .

[34]  P. Ross,et al.  The expert mind. , 2006, Scientific American.

[35]  Denis Fize,et al.  Speed of processing in the human visual system , 1996, Nature.

[36]  H. Simon,et al.  Expert chess memory: revisiting the chunking hypothesis. , 1998, Memory.

[37]  J. Martinerie,et al.  The brainweb: Phase synchronization and large-scale integration , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[38]  M. Eimer,et al.  Event-related brain potential correlates of emotional face processing , 2007, Neuropsychologia.