Pathological gamblers, with and without substance use disorders, discount delayed rewards at high rates.

Pathological gambling is classified as a disorder of impulse control, yet little research has evaluated behavioral indices of impulsivity in gamblers. The rates at which rewards delayed in time are subjectively devalued may be a behavioral marker of impulsivity. This study evaluated delay discounting in 60 pathological gamblers and 26 control participants. Gamblers were divided into those with (n = 21) and without (n = 39) substance use disorders. A hypothetical $1,000 reward was delayed at intervals ranging from 6 hr to 25 years, and immediate rewards varied from $1 to $999. Pathological gamblers discounted delayed rewards at higher rates than control participants, and gamblers with substance use disorders discounted delayed rewards at higher rates than non-substance-abusing gamblers. These data provide further evidence that rapid discounting of delayed rewards may be a feature central to impulse control and addictive disorders, including pathological gambling.

[1]  W. Mischel,et al.  Preference for delayed reward as a function of age, intelligence, and length of delay interval. , 1962, Journal of abnormal and social psychology.

[2]  G. Ainslie Specious reward: a behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. , 1975, Psychological bulletin.

[3]  R. Thaler Some empirical evidence on dynamic inconsistency , 1981 .

[4]  F. J. Evans,et al.  New Data from the Addiction Severity Index Reliability and Validity in Three Centers , 1985, The Journal of nervous and mental disease.

[5]  P. R. Pearson,et al.  Age norms for impulsiveness, venturesomeness and empathy in adults , 1985 .

[6]  A. Blaszczynski,et al.  Sensation seeking and pathological gambling. , 1986, British journal of addiction.

[7]  J. Patton,et al.  Toward a Conceptualization of Impulsivity: Components across the Behavioral and Self-Report Domains. , 1987, Multivariate behavioral research.

[8]  H. Lesieur,et al.  The South Oaks Gambling Screen (SaGS): A New Instrument for the Identification of Pathological Gamblers , 2010 .

[9]  R. McCormick,et al.  Follow-up of pathological gamblers after treatment. , 1987, The American journal of psychiatry.

[10]  J. E. Mazur An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. , 1987 .

[11]  M. Commons The effect of delay and of intervening events on reinforcement value , 2013 .

[12]  M. Dickerson,et al.  Chasing, arousal and sensation seeking in off-course gamblers. , 1987, British journal of addiction.

[13]  C C Allcock,et al.  Pathological Gamblers are Neither Impulsive Nor Sensation—Seekers , 1988, The Australian and New Zealand journal of psychiatry.

[14]  W. Mccown Multi-impulsive personality disorder and multiple substance abuse: evidence from members of self-help groups. , 1988, British journal of addiction.

[15]  H. Rachlin Why do People Gamble and Keep Gambling despite Heavy Losses? , 1990 .

[16]  D. Cross,et al.  Subjective probability and delay. , 1991, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[17]  Linda C. Sobell,et al.  Timeline Follow-Back A Technique for Assessing Self-Reported Alcohol Consumption , 1992 .

[18]  H. Lesieur,et al.  Modifying the Addiction Severity Index for Use With Pathological Gamblers , 1992 .

[19]  John P. Allen,et al.  Measuring Alcohol Consumption , 1992, Humana Press.

[20]  B. Rounsaville,et al.  Cocaine Abuse and Pathological Gambling , 1992 .

[21]  N. Sanders,et al.  Journal of behavioral decision making: "The need for contextual and technical knowledge in judgmental forecasting", 5 (1992) 39-52 , 1992 .

[22]  H. Rachlin,et al.  The effect of temporal constraints on the value of money and other commodities , 1993 .

[23]  H. Lesieur,et al.  Pathological gambling, eating disorders, and the psychoactive substance use disorders. , 1993, Journal of addictive diseases.

[24]  R. McCormick,et al.  Disinhibition and negative affectivity in substance abusers with and without a gambling problem. , 1993, Addictive behaviors.

[25]  Marjorie K. Shelley Gain/Loss Asymmetry in Risky Intertemporal Choice , 1994 .

[26]  Dc Washington Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Ed. , 1994 .

[27]  L. Green,et al.  Temporal discounting and preference reversals in choice between delayed outcomes , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[28]  L. Green,et al.  Discounting of Delayed Rewards: A Life-Span Comparison , 1994 .

[29]  B. Castellani,et al.  A comparison of pathological gamblers to alcoholics and cocaine misusers on impulsivity, sensation seeking, and craving. , 1995, The International journal of the addictions.

[30]  L. Green,et al.  Discounting of delayed rewards: Models of individual choice. , 1995, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[31]  J. Patton,et al.  Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. , 1995, Journal of clinical psychology.

[32]  H. Lesieur,et al.  Pathological gambling among methadone patients. , 1995, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[33]  Gene M. Heyman,et al.  Resolving the contradictions of addiction , 1996, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[34]  K. Kirby,et al.  Delay-discounting probabilistic rewards: Rates decrease as amounts increase , 1996, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[35]  M. Pato,et al.  Obsessive-compulsive disorder in patients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. , 1997, The American journal of psychiatry.

[36]  R E Tremblay,et al.  Dispositional predictors of problem gambling in male adolescents. , 1997, The American journal of psychiatry.

[37]  W K Bickel,et al.  Impulsive and self-control choices in opioid-dependent patients and non-drug-using control participants: drug and monetary rewards. , 1997, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.

[38]  Z. Steel,et al.  Impulsivity in pathological gambling: the antisocial impulsivist. , 1997, Addiction.

[39]  Z. Steel,et al.  Impulsivity, personality disorders and pathological gambling severity. , 1998, Addiction.

[40]  F. Moeller,et al.  Impulsivity and history of drug dependence. , 1998, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[41]  R. Vuchinich,et al.  Hyperbolic temporal discounting in social drinkers and problem drinkers. , 1998, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.

[42]  W. Bickel,et al.  Shortened time horizons and insensitivity to future consequences in heroin addicts. , 1998, Addiction.

[43]  A L Bretteville-Jensen,et al.  Addiction and discounting. , 1999, Journal of health economics.

[44]  W. Bickel,et al.  Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[45]  A. Odum,et al.  Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: delay discounting in current, never, and ex-smokers , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[46]  L. Arseneault,et al.  Impulsivity predicts problem gambling in low SES adolescent males. , 1999, Addiction.

[47]  J. Vander Bilt,et al.  Estimating the prevalence of disordered gambling behavior in the United States and Canada: a research synthesis. , 1999, American journal of public health.

[48]  N. Petry,et al.  Prevalence, assessment, and treatment of pathological gambling: a review. , 1999, Psychiatric services.

[49]  N. Petry,et al.  Excessive discounting of delayed rewards in substance abusers with gambling problems. , 1999, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[50]  N. Petry,et al.  Delay discounting of money and alcohol in actively using alcoholics, currently abstinent alcoholics, and controls , 2001, Psychopharmacology.

[51]  N. Petry,et al.  Substance abuse, pathological gambling, and impulsiveness. , 2001, Drug and alcohol dependence.