When seeing outweighs feeling: a role for prefrontal cortex in passive control of negative affect in blindsight

Affective neuroscience has been strongly influenced by the view that a ‘feeling’ is the perception of somatic changes and has consequently often neglected the neural mechanisms that underlie the integration of somatic and other information in affective experience. Here, we investigate affective processing by means of functional magnetic resonance imaging in nine cortically blind patients. In these patients, unilateral postgeniculate lesions prevent primary cortical visual processing in part of the visual field which, as a result, becomes subjectively blind. Residual subcortical processing of visual information, however, is assumed to occur in the entire visual field. As we have reported earlier, these patients show significant startle reflex potentiation when a threat-related visual stimulus is shown in their blind visual field. Critically, this was associated with an increase of brain activity in somatosensory-related areas, and an increase in experienced negative affect. Here, we investigated the patients’ response when the visual stimulus was shown in the sighted visual field, that is, when it was visible and cortically processed. Despite the fact that startle reflex potentiation was similar in the blind and sighted visual field, patients reported significantly less negative affect during stimulation of the sighted visual field. In other words, when the visual stimulus was visible and received full cortical processing, the patients’ phenomenal experience of affect did not closely reflect somatic changes. This decoupling of phenomenal affective experience and somatic changes was associated with an increase of activity in the left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and a decrease of affect-related somatosensory activity. Moreover, patients who showed stronger left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex activity tended to show a stronger decrease of affect-related somatosensory activity. Our findings show that similar affective somatic changes can be associated with different phenomenal experiences of affect, depending on the depth of cortical processing. They are in line with a model in which the left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex is a relay station that integrates information about subcortically triggered somatic responses and information resulting from in-depth cortical stimulus processing. Tentatively, we suggest that the observed decoupling of somatic responses and experienced affect, and the reduction of negative phenomenal experience, can be explained by a left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex-mediated inhibition of affect-related somatosensory activity.

[1]  U. Dimberg,et al.  Unconscious Facial Reactions to Emotional Facial Expressions , 2000, Psychological science.

[2]  R. Zajonc,et al.  Additivity of nonconscious affect: combined effects of priming and exposure. , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[3]  N Birbaumer,et al.  Startle reflex and emotion modulation impairment after a right amygdala lesion. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[4]  J. Gross,et al.  The cognitive control of emotion , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[5]  Nikolaus Weiskopf,et al.  Infrared oculography—validation of a new method to monitor startle eyeblink amplitudes during fMRI , 2004, NeuroImage.

[6]  L Weiskrantz,et al.  Unseen stimuli modulate conscious visual experience: evidence from inter-hemispheric summation , 2001, Neuroreport.

[7]  Christopher J Rennie,et al.  Mode of Functional Connectivity in Amygdala Pathways Dissociates Level of Awareness for Signals of Fear , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A unified statistical approach for determining significant signals in images of cerebral activation , 1996, Human brain mapping.

[9]  A. Ohman,et al.  Fear and the amygdala: manipulation of awareness generates differential cerebral responses to phobic and fear-relevant (but nonfeared) stimuli. , 2004, Emotion.

[10]  M. Davis,et al.  Enhancement of acoustic startle by electrical stimulation of the amygdala. , 1988, Behavioral neuroscience.

[11]  Sean A Spence,et al.  The deceptive brain. , 2004, Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine.

[12]  Brice A. Kuhl,et al.  Neural Systems Underlying the Suppression of Unwanted Memories , 2004, Science.

[13]  S. Southwick,et al.  Neural correlates of memories of childhood sexual abuse in women with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. , 1999, The American journal of psychiatry.

[14]  M. Bradley,et al.  Looking at pictures: affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral reactions. , 1993, Psychophysiology.

[15]  Harald T Schupp,et al.  Affective blindsight: intact fear conditioning to a visual cue in a cortically blind patient. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[16]  Asaid Khateb,et al.  Discriminating emotional faces without primary visual cortices involves the right amygdala , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[17]  M. Davis,et al.  Lesions of the perirhinal cortex but not of the frontal, medial prefrontal, visual, or insular cortex block fear-potentiated startle using a visual conditioned stimulus , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[18]  R. Dolan,et al.  A subcortical pathway to the right amygdala mediating "unseen" fear. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[19]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  Rethinking Feelings: An fMRI Study of the Cognitive Regulation of Emotion , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[20]  William A. Cunningham,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Separable Neural Components in the Processing of Black and White , 2022 .

[21]  G. Pagnoni,et al.  Explicit and Incidental Facial Expression Processing: An fMRI Study , 2001, NeuroImage.

[22]  A. Öhman,et al.  Automatically elicited fear: Conditioned skin conductance responses to masked facial expressions , 1994 .

[23]  M. Vangel,et al.  Brain Activity Associated with Expectancy-Enhanced Placebo Analgesia as Measured by Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  R. Dolan,et al.  Unconscious fear influences emotional awareness of faces and voices. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[25]  R. Dolan,et al.  How the Brain Translates Money into Force: A Neuroimaging Study of Subliminal Motivation , 2007, Science.

[26]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Reciprocal modulation and attenuation in the prefrontal cortex: An fMRI study on emotional–cognitive interaction , 2004, Human brain mapping.

[27]  T. Dietrich,et al.  Placebo in Emotional Processing— Induced Expectations of Anxiety Relief Activate a Generalized Modulatory Network , 2005, Neuron.

[28]  Marco Tamietto,et al.  Affective blindsight in the intact brain: Neural interhemispheric summation for unseen fearful expressions , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  K. Scherer What are emotions? And how can they be measured? , 2005 .

[30]  Lisa M Shin,et al.  Exaggerated amygdala response to masked facial stimuli in posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional MRI study , 2000, Biological Psychiatry.

[31]  V. Lamme,et al.  Repression of unconscious information by conscious processing: evidence from affective blindsight induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[32]  Charles S. Carver,et al.  Influence of Subliminal Visual Images on the Experience of Anxiety , 1987 .

[33]  Kent C. Berridge,et al.  Unconscious Affective Reactions to Masked Happy Versus Angry Faces Influence Consumption Behavior and Judgments of Value , 2005, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[34]  Bruce W. Smith,et al.  Modulation of emotion by cognition and cognition by emotion , 2007, NeuroImage.

[35]  M. W. Greenlee,et al.  MR-Eyetracker: a new method for eye movement recording in functional magnetic resonance imaging , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[36]  R. Davidson,et al.  Suppression and enhancement of emotional responses to unpleasant pictures. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[37]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Parsing executive processes: strategic vs. evaluative functions of the anterior cingulate cortex. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[38]  Atsushi Sato,et al.  Association of neural and physiological responses during voluntary emotion suppression , 2006, NeuroImage.

[39]  C. Cavada,et al.  The anatomical connections of the macaque monkey orbitofrontal cortex. A review. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[40]  Y. Miyashita,et al.  Common inhibitory mechanism in human inferior prefrontal cortex revealed by event-related functional MRI. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[41]  M. Beauregard,et al.  Neural Correlates of Conscious Self-Regulation of Emotion , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[42]  M. Erb,et al.  Processing of a simple aversive conditioned stimulus in a divided visual field paradigm: an fMRI study , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[43]  H. Pape,et al.  Direct synaptic connections of axons from superior colliculus with identified thalamo‐amygdaloid projection neurons in the rat: Possible substrates of a subcortical visual pathway to the amygdala , 1999, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[44]  Michael Erb,et al.  Parietal somatosensory association cortex mediates affective blindsight , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[45]  P. Kleinginna,et al.  A categorized list of emotion definitions, with suggestions for a consensual definition , 1981 .

[46]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Dissociable prefrontal brain systems for attention and emotion , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[47]  J. Duncan,et al.  Prefrontal cortical function and anxiety: controlling attention to threat-related stimuli , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[48]  R. Zajonc,et al.  Affect, cognition, and awareness: affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[49]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex , 1999, Nature.

[50]  J. Russell Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. , 2003, Psychological review.

[51]  Andrew D. Engell,et al.  The Neural Bases of Cognitive Conflict and Control in Moral Judgment , 2004, Neuron.

[52]  M. Bradley,et al.  Measuring emotion: the Self-Assessment Manikin and the Semantic Differential. , 1994, Journal of behavior therapy and experimental psychiatry.

[53]  L Weiskrantz,et al.  Non-conscious recognition of affect in the absence of striate cortex. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[54]  K. Phan,et al.  Neural substrates for voluntary suppression of negative affect: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study , 2005, Biological Psychiatry.

[55]  J. Singer,et al.  Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. , 1962, Psychological review.

[56]  T. Shallice,et al.  Human cingulate cortex and autonomic control: converging neuroimaging and clinical evidence. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[57]  Raymond J. Dolan,et al.  Levels of appraisal: A medial prefrontal role in high-level appraisal of emotional material , 2006, NeuroImage.

[58]  A. McDonald Cortical pathways to the mammalian amygdala , 1998, Progress in Neurobiology.

[59]  Raymond J. Dolan,et al.  Anxiety Reduction through Detachment: Subjective, Physiological, and Neural Effects , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[60]  L. Weiskrantz,et al.  Differential extrageniculostriate and amygdala responses to presentation of emotional faces in a cortically blind field. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[61]  K. LaBar,et al.  Startle Modulation during Conscious Emotion Regulation Is Arousal-dependent , 2022 .

[62]  C Tempelmann,et al.  Electrodynamic headphones and woofers for application in magnetic resonance imaging scanners. , 1998, Medical physics.

[63]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Placebo-Induced Changes in fMRI in the Anticipation and Experience of Pain , 2004, Science.

[64]  M. Livingstone,et al.  Neuronal correlates of visibility and invisibility in the primate visual system , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[65]  Simon B. Eickhoff,et al.  A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data , 2005, NeuroImage.

[66]  Nouchine Hadjikhani,et al.  Non-conscious recognition of emotional body language , 2006, Neuroreport.

[67]  J. D. E. Gabrieli,et al.  Integration of diverse information in working memory within the frontal lobe , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[68]  W. James,et al.  What Is an Emotion , 1977 .

[69]  Kevin N. Ochsner,et al.  For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion , 2004, NeuroImage.

[70]  R. Veit,et al.  Regulation of emotional responses elicited by threat‐related stimuli , 2007, Human brain mapping.

[71]  A. Sahraie,et al.  Temporal properties of spatial channel of processing in hemianopia , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[72]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Neural responses to salient visual stimuli , 1997, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.