The audience who knew too much: investigating the role of spontaneous theory of mind on the processing of dramatic irony scenes in film

As in real life, cinema viewers rely on spontaneous theory of mind (SToM) to interpret characters' mental states. Thus, analyzing cinematic structures offers a unique opportunity to examine ecologically valid sociocognitive processes. We conducted a proof-of-concept study (N = 42) to explore how SToM inferences impact film event comprehension in dramatic irony scenes, where knowledge divergence exists between the audience and characters. We hypothesized that spectators would focus more on characters' mental states in such false-belief inducing scenarios compared to scenarios without such disparity. We used six Harold Lloyd silent comedy clips in a narrative comprehension and spontaneous mental state attribution study with a between-subject (Knowledge Manipulation: Installation vs. Control) and within-subject (Phase: Context vs. Exploitation) comparisons. We provided critical information unknown to the characters only to the Installation group and withheld it from the Control group. By comparing differences in participants' descriptions of the clips during the Context phase (varying across groups) and Exploitation phase (same across groups), we evaluated viewers' processing of the same scenes based on their false- or true-belief representations. Our findings indicate that the Installation group used more cognitive mental state words during the Exploitation phase relative to the Context phase, suggesting that exposure to undisclosed critical information enhances the frequency of spontaneous epistemic state inferences and integration into event models of the exploitation. This research advances neurocinematics by highlighting spontaneous sociocognitive processes in event perception and comprehension and provides a novel dramatic irony film corpus and measures for future moment-to-moment SToM processing studies across cognitive-behavioral, physiological, and neural levels.

[1]  E. Finn,et al.  Leveraging the power of media to drive cognition: a media-informed approach to naturalistic neuroscience , 2022, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[2]  D. Perry,et al.  Somatic engagement alters subsequent neurobehavioral correlates of affective mentalizing , 2021, Human brain mapping.

[3]  Laurie R Santos,et al.  Knowledge before belief , 2020, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[4]  M. Milham,et al.  Towards clinical applications of movie fMRI , 2020, NeuroImage.

[5]  Marcel Brass,et al.  Defining the neural correlates of spontaneous theory of mind (ToM): An fMRI multi-study investigation , 2019, NeuroImage.

[6]  Lester C. Loschky,et al.  The Scene Perception & Event Comprehension Theory (SPECT) Applied to Visual Narratives , 2019, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[7]  Jo K. Evershed,et al.  Gorilla in our midst: An online behavioral experiment builder , 2019, Behavior Research Methods.

[8]  Punit Shah,et al.  Recent Advances and New Directions in Measuring Theory of Mind in Autistic Adults , 2018, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[9]  G. Sideridis,et al.  Measuring Individual Differences in Cognitive, Affective, and Spontaneous Theory of Mind Among School-Aged Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2018, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.

[10]  Thomas Klausch,et al.  Watching Closely: Shot Scale Influences Theory of Mind Response in Visual Narratives , 2018, J. Media Psychol. Theor. Methods Appl..

[11]  D. W. Nelson Solving interpersonal problems when fatigued: The beneficial effect of positive mood , 2018, Social Behavior and Personality: An international journal.

[12]  Philipp Kanske,et al.  On the interaction of social affect and cognition: empathy, compassion and theory of mind , 2018, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.

[13]  B. Rooney,et al.  Watching More Closely: Shot Scale Affects Film Viewers’ Theory of Mind Tendency But Not Ability , 2018, Front. Psychol..

[14]  Robert H. Wilson Director , 2018, Rediscovering Stanislavsky.

[15]  Lester C. Loschky,et al.  What is the role of the film viewer? The effects of narrative comprehension and viewing task on gaze control in film , 2017, Cognitive research: principles and implications.

[16]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  The emergence of recursion in human language: Mentalising predicts recursive syntax task performance , 2017, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[17]  A. Meyer-Lindenberg,et al.  Differential responses of the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex and right posterior superior temporal sulcus to spontaneous mentalizing , 2017, Human brain mapping.

[18]  Uri Hasson,et al.  Same Story, Different Story , 2017, Psychological science.

[19]  Annabel D. Nijhof,et al.  Brain activation for spontaneous and explicit false belief tasks overlaps: new fMRI evidence on belief processing and violation of expectation , 2016, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[20]  Laura K. Taylor,et al.  Empathy: A Review of the Concept , 2016 .

[21]  E. Redcay,et al.  Spontaneous mentalizing captures variability in the cortical thickness of social brain regions. , 2015, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[22]  Mikko Sams,et al.  Synchronous brain activity across individuals underlies shared psychological perspectives , 2014, NeuroImage.

[23]  S. Brown-Schmidt,et al.  Do Adults Show a Curse of Knowledge in False-Belief Reasoning? A Robust Estimate of the True Effect Size , 2014, PloS one.

[24]  C. Heyes Submentalizing: I Am Not Really Reading Your Mind , 2014, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[25]  J. Sommerville,et al.  Measuring beliefs in centimeters: private knowledge biases preschoolers' and adults' representation of others' beliefs. , 2013, Child development.

[26]  Rebecca Saxe,et al.  Similar Brain Activation during False Belief Tasks in a Large Sample of Adults with and without Autism , 2013, PloS one.

[27]  Rory T. Devine,et al.  Silent films and strange stories: theory of mind, gender, and social experiences in middle childhood. , 2013, Child development.

[28]  Alicia M. Hymel,et al.  Belief, Desire, Action, and Other Stuff: Theory of Mind in Movies , 2013 .

[29]  P. Jackson,et al.  On what ground do we mentalize? Characteristics of current tasks and sources of information that contribute to mentalizing judgments. , 2013, Psychological assessment.

[30]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  The Big Picture: Storytelling Ability in Adults with Autism Spectrum Conditions , 2012, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[31]  K. Ochsner,et al.  The neuroscience of empathy: progress, pitfalls and promise , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.

[32]  Atsushi Senju,et al.  Spontaneous Theory of Mind and Its Absence in Autism Spectrum Disorders , 2012, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[33]  I. Apperly What is “theory of mind”? Concepts, cognitive processes and individual differences , 2012, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[34]  I. Dziobek Comment: Towards a More Ecologically Valid Assessment of Empathy , 2012 .

[35]  J. Sommerville,et al.  Theory of Mind Through the Ages: Older and Middle-Aged Adults Exhibit More Errors Than Do Younger Adults on a Continuous False Belief Task , 2011, Experimental aging research.

[36]  S. Shamay-Tsoory,et al.  Neuroanatomical and neurochemical bases of theory of mind , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[37]  Melanie R. Glenwright,et al.  Processing of Ironic Language in Children with High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2011, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[38]  L. Phillips,et al.  Lifespan aging and belief reasoning: Influences of executive function and social cue decoding , 2011, Cognition.

[39]  Melanie R. Glenwright,et al.  Children and Adults Understand That Verbal Irony Interpretation Depends on Listener Knowledge , 2011 .

[40]  Ian A. Apperly,et al.  Two sources of evidence on the non-automaticity of true and false belief ascription , 2010, Cognition.

[41]  R. Baillargeon,et al.  False-belief understanding in infants , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[42]  E. Erdfelder,et al.  Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses , 2009, Behavior research methods.

[43]  S. Brown-Schmidt,et al.  The role of executive function in perspective taking during online language comprehension , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[44]  R. Baillargeon,et al.  Which penguin is this? Attributing false beliefs about object identity at 18 months. , 2009, Child development.

[45]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Moral Dilemmas Film Task: a study of spontaneous narratives by individuals with autism spectrum conditions , 2009, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.

[46]  Oliver T Wolf,et al.  Dissociation of Cognitive and Emotional Empathy in Adults with Asperger Syndrome Using the Multifaceted Empathy Test (MET) , 2008, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[47]  Simone G. Shamay-Tsoory,et al.  Dissociable prefrontal networks for cognitive and affective theory of mind: A lesion study , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[48]  Paul Bloom,et al.  The Curse of Knowledge in Reasoning About False Beliefs , 2007, Psychological science.

[49]  Jessica A. Hehman,et al.  Representational and executive selection resources in ‘theory of mind’: Evidence from compromised belief-desire reasoning in old age , 2006, Cognition.

[50]  Jan K. Woike,et al.  Introducing MASC: A Movie for the Assessment of Social Cognition , 2006, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[51]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  The “Reading the Mind in Films” Task: Complex emotion recognition in adults with and without autism spectrum conditions , 2006, Social neuroscience.

[52]  Lisa Zunshine,et al.  Why We Read Fiction: Theory of Mind and the Novel , 2006 .

[53]  R. Poldrack Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data? , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[54]  J. Perner,et al.  Do infants really understand false belief? Response to Leslie , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[55]  K. Krippendorff Reliability in Content Analysis: Some Common Misconceptions and Recommendations , 2004 .

[56]  Cynthia T. F. Klein,et al.  Motivational Antecedents of Empathy: Inhibiting Effects of Fatigue , 2003 .

[57]  H. Wellman,et al.  Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. , 2001, Child development.

[58]  P. Bloom,et al.  Two reasons to abandon the false belief task as a test of theory of mind , 2000, Cognition.

[59]  A. Klin Attributing social meaning to ambiguous visual stimuli in higher-functioning autism and Asperger syndrome: The Social Attribution Task. , 2000, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[60]  C. Frith,et al.  Movement and Mind: A Functional Imaging Study of Perception and Interpretation of Complex Intentional Movement Patterns , 2000, NeuroImage.

[61]  M. Rutter,et al.  The Awkward Moments Test: A Naturalistic Measure of Social Understanding in Autism , 2000, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[62]  Jeffrey T. Hancock,et al.  Children's Comprehension of Critical and Complimentary Forms of Verbal Irony , 2000 .

[63]  C. Frith,et al.  Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[64]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  Theory-of-mind deficits and causal attributions , 1998 .

[65]  Rolf A. Zwaan,et al.  Situation models in language comprehension and memory. , 1998, Psychological bulletin.

[66]  E. Tan,et al.  Emotion and the Structure of Narrative Film: Film As An Emotion Machine , 1995 .

[67]  Alan M. Leslie,et al.  Domain specificity in conceptual development: Neuropsychological evidence from autism , 1992, Cognition.

[68]  J. Fodor A theory of the child's theory of mind , 1992, Cognition.

[69]  K. Kaye The Kid Brother , 1992 .

[70]  Alison Gopnik,et al.  Theoretical explanations of children's understanding of the mind , 1991 .

[71]  D. Zaitchik,et al.  When representations conflict with reality: The preschooler's problem with false beliefs and “false” photographs , 1990, Cognition.

[72]  A. Gopnik,et al.  Children's understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearance-reality distinction. , 1988, Child development.

[73]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind” ? , 1985, Cognition.

[74]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception , 1983, Cognition.

[75]  D. Premack,et al.  Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? , 1978, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[76]  Christopher J. Honey,et al.  Same Story , Different Story : The Neural Representation of Interpretive Frameworks , 2017 .

[77]  Neil Cohn From Visual Narrative Grammar to Filmic Narrative Grammar: The narrative structure of static and moving images , 2016 .

[78]  J. Astington,et al.  Further development in social reasoning revealed in discourse irony understanding. , 2008, Child development.

[79]  James Stiller,et al.  Perspective-taking and memory capacity predict social network size , 2007, Soc. Networks.

[80]  U. Frith,et al.  Do triangles play tricks? Attribution of mental states to animated shapes in normal and abnormal development , 2000 .

[81]  E. Tan Emotion and the structure of narrative film , 1996 .

[82]  Murray Smith Engaging Characters: Fiction, Emotion, and the Cinema , 1995 .

[83]  J. Dalen Perspectives … , 1968, International nursing review.