Are children’s spontaneous number focusing tendencies related to their home numeracy environment?

Young children show large individual differences in their tendency to focus spontaneously on numerical aspects (e.g., numerosities or Arabic number symbols) of their everyday environment. The origins of these individual differences are unclear. Given the role of the home numeracy environment (HNE) in children’s early mathematical development and the assumed link between children’s spontaneous number focusing tendencies and their numerical behavior in everyday life, it is plausible that children’s spontaneous focusing tendencies are related to their HNE. The present study aimed to test this hypothesis by longitudinally investigating children’s spontaneous focusing on numerosity (SFON) and spontaneous focusing on Arabic number symbols (SFONS) in relation to their HNE. Participants were 128 children (4- to 5-year-olds), who were followed from the second until the third year of Flemish kindergarten. In both kindergarten years, children completed a SFON and SFONS Picture task while their parents completed a home numeracy questionnaire. Correlation analyses and structural equation modeling revealed no significant associations between children’s spontaneous number focusing tendencies and their HNE, neither in second nor in third year of kindergarten. This finding suggests that children’s spontaneous number focusing tendencies are not per se related to their HNE. Various possible explanations for this unexpected finding are discussed and directions for further research on this relationship are suggested.

[1]  B. Reynvoet,et al.  Frequency of Home Numeracy Activities Is Differentially Related to Basic Number Processing and Calculation Skills in Kindergartners , 2018, Front. Psychol..

[2]  Randall M. Jones,et al.  Family Home Care Providers’ and Parents’ Beliefs and Practices Concerning Mathematics with Young Children , 2000 .

[3]  Lieven Verschaffel,et al.  Verbal and action‐based measures of kindergartners' SFON and their associations with number‐related utterances during picture book reading , 2018, The British journal of educational psychology.

[4]  Rebecca Bull,et al.  Executive Functioning and Mathematics Achievement , 2014 .

[5]  J. Millán,et al.  Novice Shooters With Lower Pre-shooting Alpha Power Have Better Performance During Competition in a Virtual Reality Scenario , 2018, Front. Psychol..

[6]  Erno Lehtinen,et al.  Spontaneous Focusing on Numerosity and Mathematical Skills of Young Children. , 2005 .

[7]  P. Bentler,et al.  Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis : Conventional criteria versus new alternatives , 1999 .

[8]  George K. Georgiou,et al.  Cross-lagged relationships between home learning environment and academic achievement in Chinese , 2015 .

[9]  E. Lehtinen,et al.  Preschool spontaneous focusing on numerosity predicts rational number conceptual knowledge 6 years later , 2015 .

[10]  Matthew Inglis,et al.  Spontaneous focusing on numerosity and the arithmetic advantage , 2015 .

[11]  Erno Lehtinen,et al.  Development of Counting Skills: Role of Spontaneous Focusing on Numerosity and Subitizing-Based Enumeration , 2007 .

[12]  Nancy C. Jordan,et al.  Early math matters: kindergarten number competence and later mathematics outcomes. , 2009, Developmental psychology.

[13]  Lieven Verschaffel,et al.  Spontaneous focusing on Arabic number symbols and its association with early mathematical competencies , 2019, Early Childhood Research Quarterly.

[14]  G. Duncan,et al.  School readiness and later achievement. , 2007, Developmental psychology.

[15]  C. Hulme,et al.  Early language and executive skills predict variations in number and arithmetic skills in children at family-risk of dyslexia and typically developing controls , 2015, Learning and instruction.

[16]  David J. Purpura,et al.  Understanding the Home Math Environment and Its Role in Predicting Parent Report of Children’s Math Skills , 2016, PloS one.

[17]  S. Skwarchuk,et al.  How Do Parents Support Preschoolers’ Numeracy Learning Experiences at Home? , 2009 .

[18]  Heather J. Bachman,et al.  How Do Parents Foster Young Children's Math Skills? , 2018 .

[19]  Christopher B. Wolfe,et al.  Which Preschool Mathematics Competencies Are Most Predictive of Fifth Grade Achievement? , 2016, Early childhood research quarterly.

[20]  David J. Purpura,et al.  Age‐related differences in the relation between the home numeracy environment and numeracy skills , 2017 .

[21]  Michael Schneider,et al.  Associations of non-symbolic and symbolic numerical magnitude processing with mathematical competence: a meta-analysis. , 2017, Developmental science.

[22]  L. Verschaffel,et al.  Spontaneous focusing on numerosity: A review of recent research , 2016 .

[23]  Geetha B. Ramani,et al.  Math talk during informal learning activities in Head Start families , 2015 .

[24]  M. Hannula-Sormunen Spontaneous Focusing On Numerosity and its Relation to Counting and Arithmetic , 2015 .

[25]  James B. Schreiber,et al.  Reporting Structural Equation Modeling and Confirmatory Factor Analysis Results: A Review , 2006 .

[26]  S. Wimbarti,et al.  Indonesian Children's Play with Their Mothers and Older Siblings. , 1995 .

[27]  B. Blevins-Knabe,et al.  Early Mathematical Development: How the Home Environment Matters , 2016 .

[28]  P. Davis-Kean,et al.  Maternal Math Talk in the Home and Math Skills in Preschool Children , 2016 .

[29]  Jake McMullen,et al.  Spontaneous Mathematical Focusing Tendencies in Mathematical Development and Education , 2018, Constructing Number.

[30]  G. Caskie,et al.  Home Numeracy Environments of Preschoolers: Examining Relations Among Mathematical Activities, Parent Mathematical Beliefs, and Early Mathematical Skills , 2015 .

[31]  L. Verhoeven,et al.  Child and Home Predictors of Early Numeracy Skills in Kindergarten. , 2012 .

[32]  J. Kane A user’ guide to clozapine , 2011, Acta psychiatrica Scandinavica.

[33]  E. Lehtinen,et al.  Preschool Children’s Spontaneous Focusing on Numerosity, Subitizing, and Counting Skills as Predictors of Their Mathematical Performance Seven Years Later at School , 2015 .

[34]  Geetha B. Ramani,et al.  Parents' Estimations of Preschoolers' Number Skills Relate to at‐Home Number‐Related Activity Engagement , 2017 .

[35]  Hannah M. Nash,et al.  Are the literacy difficulties that characterize developmental dyslexia associated with a failure to integrate letters and speech sounds? , 2016, Developmental science.

[36]  Erno Lehtinen,et al.  Spontaneous focusing on numerosity as a domain-specific predictor of arithmetical skills. , 2010, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[37]  Xiao Zhang,et al.  The contribution of parent–child numeracy activities to young Chinese children's mathematical ability , 2017, The British journal of educational psychology.

[38]  Parent–child mathematical interactions: examining self-report and direct observation , 2017 .

[39]  Charlie Huveneers,et al.  How Close is too Close? The Effect of a Non-Lethal Electric Shark Deterrent on White Shark Behaviour , 2016, PloS one.

[40]  L. Verschaffel,et al.  Development of SFON in Ecuadorian Kindergartners , 2017 .

[41]  Jeffrey Bisanz,et al.  Home numeracy experiences and children’s math performance in the early school years. , 2009 .

[42]  B. Reynvoet,et al.  Investigating the relationship between two home numeracy measures: A questionnaire and observations during Lego building and book reading , 2018, The British journal of developmental psychology.

[43]  Jo-Anne LeFevre,et al.  Formal and informal home learning activities in relation to children's early numeracy and literacy skills: the development of a home numeracy model. , 2014, Journal of experimental child psychology.