Children's Understanding of Drivers' Intentions

To become more skilled as pedestrians, children need to acquire a view of the traffic environment as one in which road users are active agents with different intentions and objectives. This paper describes a simulation study designed to explore children's understanding of drivers' intentions. It also investigated the effect of training children's sensitivity, through peer discussion and adult guidance, to the cues by which drivers signal their intentions. Results confirmed that children's ability to accurately predict drivers' intentions improves with age and that sensitizing children through training to the options for action available to drivers when signalling a manoeuvre improves their accuracy in predicting drivers' intentions. Training was also found to shift children's focus from contextual infrastructural features of the traffic environment (e.g. traffic signals, stop signs) by which to judge drivers' likely intentions to the explicit cues that drivers use to signal their imminent actions (e.g. slowing down, moving into the kerb). Training on the simulation was also shown to transfer to practical decision making at the roadside.

[1]  M Malek,et al.  The epidemiology and prevention of child pedestrian injury. , 1990, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[2]  Andrew Tolmie,et al.  The effects of adult guidance and peer discussion on the development of children's representations: evidence from the training of pedestrian skills. , 2005, British journal of psychology.

[3]  Thomas R. Shultz,et al.  Development of the ability to distinguish intended actions from mistakes, reflexes, and passive movements , 1980 .

[4]  M. Kerr,et al.  Parental monitoring: a reinterpretation. , 2000, Child development.

[5]  D. Yagil Beliefs, motives and situational factors related to pedestrians' self-reported behavior at signal-controlled crossings , 2000 .

[6]  Penny Munn,et al.  Development of justification in disputes with mother and sibling. , 1987 .

[7]  Z. Tabibi,et al.  Choosing a safe place to cross the road: the relationship between attention and identification of safe and dangerous road-crossing sites. , 2003, Child: care, health and development.

[8]  Andrew Tolmie,et al.  Gender and Dialogue in Secondary School Physics. , 1993 .

[9]  J. Thomson,et al.  Influence of virtual reality training on the roadside crossing judgments of child pedestrians. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[10]  Charles Vlek,et al.  Essential psychology for environmental policy making , 2000 .

[11]  W. Damon,et al.  Critical distinctions among three approaches to peer education , 1989 .

[12]  E R Hoffmann,et al.  Children's Estimates of Vehicle Approach Times , 1980, Human factors.

[13]  J A Rothengatter,et al.  Blueprint of an analysis of the pedestrian's task—I , 1981 .

[14]  Ronald R. Mourant,et al.  Strategies of Visual Search by Novice and Experienced Drivers , 1972 .

[15]  C. Howe,et al.  Peer Collaboration and Conceptual Growth in Physics: Task Influences on Children's Understanding of Heating and Cooling , 1995 .

[16]  George L. Dunbar,et al.  Young children's concepts of danger , 2000 .

[17]  C I Howarth,et al.  An analysis of road accidents involving child pedestrians. , 1974, Ergonomics.

[18]  J. Thomson,et al.  Children's perception of safety and danger on the road. , 1991, British journal of psychology.

[19]  K. Ampofo-Boateng,et al.  Child pedestrian accidents: a case for preventive medicine , 1990 .

[20]  D. Whitebread,et al.  The contribution of visual search strategies to the development of pedestrian skills by 4-11 year-old children. , 2000, The British journal of educational psychology.

[21]  Andrew Tolmie,et al.  Conceptual change in biology: Group interaction and the understanding of inheritance , 2000 .