Behavioral and Functional Neuroimaging Evidence for Prefrontal Dysfunction in Methamphetamine-Dependent Subjects

Stimulant-dependent subjects show dysfunctions in decision-making similar to those seen in subjects with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Studies of drug craving, reward association, and decision-making have implicated dysfunctions of the dorsolateral and orbitofrontal cortex as a key neural substrate in subjects with stimulant dependence. Here, a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study was carried out to determine the relationship between decision-making dysfunction and neural activation in different prefrontal areas. This investigation tested the behavioral hypothesis that methamphetamine-dependent subjects in early sustained remission show decision-making dysfunctions that are consistent with an increased reliance on stimulus-contingent response selection. It was hypothesized that these decision-making dysfunctions are due to differences in task-related activation in the dorsolateral and ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Ten methamphetamine-dependent subjects were compared with ten age- and education-matched controls performing a two-choice prediction task and a two-choice response task during a fMRI session. Response bias, latency, and mutual information measures assessing the underlying strategies of the decision-making sequences were obtained. First, methamphetamine-dependent subjects were more influenced by the immediately preceding outcome during the two-choice prediction task relative to normal comparison subjects. Second, methamphetamine-dependent subjects activated less dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (BA 9) and failed to activate ventromedial cortex (BA 10,11) during the two-choice prediction task compared with the two-choice response task. These results support the basic hypothesis that stimulant-dependent subjects exhibit fundamental cognitive deficits during decision-making that are consistent with both orbitofrontal and dorsolateral prefrontal dysfunction.

[1]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Neural correlates of a decision in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the macaque , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[2]  M. First,et al.  The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R (SCID). I: History, rationale, and description. , 1992, Archives of general psychiatry.

[3]  R. Elliott,et al.  Ventromedial prefrontal cortex mediates guessing , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[4]  E. Rolls,et al.  Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[5]  L Green,et al.  Amount of reward has opposite effects on the discounting of delayed and probabilistic outcomes. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[6]  G. Bartzokis,et al.  Abstinence from Cocaine Reduces High-Risk Responses on a Gambling Task , 2000, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[7]  R. Elliott,et al.  Activation of Different Anterior Cingulate Foci in Association with Hypothesis Testing and Response Selection , 1998, NeuroImage.

[8]  W. Bickel,et al.  Shortened time horizons and insensitivity to future consequences in heroin addicts. , 1998, Addiction.

[9]  Michael L. Platt,et al.  Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal cortex , 1999, Nature.

[10]  N. Volkow,et al.  Decreased dopamine D2 receptor availability is associated with reduced frontal metabolism in cocaine abusers , 1993, Synapse.

[11]  Mark S. Cohen,et al.  Parametric Analysis of fMRI Data Using Linear Systems Methods , 1997, NeuroImage.

[12]  Carlo Contoreggi,et al.  Drug abusers show impaired performance in a laboratory test of decision making , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  J. Monterosso,et al.  Beyond discounting: possible experimental models of impulse control , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[14]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Willed action and the prefrontal cortex in man: a study with PET , 1991, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[15]  J. Talairach,et al.  Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging , 1988 .

[16]  M. Jahanshahi,et al.  The left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and random generation of responses: studies with transcranial magnetic stimulation , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  A. Damasio,et al.  Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex , 1994, Cognition.

[18]  T. Robbins,et al.  Dissociable Deficits in the Decision-Making Cognition of Chronic Amphetamine Abusers, Opiate Abusers, Patients with Focal Damage to Prefrontal Cortex, and Tryptophan-Depleted Normal Volunteers: Evidence for Monoaminergic Mechanisms , 1999, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[19]  T. Robbins,et al.  Contrasting Cortical and Subcortical Activations Produced by Attentional-Set Shifting and Reversal Learning in Humans , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[20]  A. Tversky,et al.  Contingent weighting in judgment and choice , 1988 .

[21]  E T Rolls,et al.  Sensory‐specific satiety‐related olfactory activation of the human orbitofrontal cortex , 2000, Neuroreport.

[22]  T. Robbins,et al.  Specific cognitive deficits in mild frontal variant frontotemporal dementia. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[23]  M. Ernst,et al.  Orbitofrontal cortex and human drug abuse: functional imaging. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[24]  M. Geyer,et al.  The assessment of sequential response organization in schizophrenic and control subjects , 1994, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry.

[25]  D. Wong,et al.  Reduced Striatal Dopamine Transporter Density in Abstinent Methamphetamine and Methcathinone Users: Evidence from Positron Emission Tomography Studies with [11C]WIN-35,428 , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  R. J. Dolan,et al.  Differential neural response to positive and negative feedback in planning and guessing tasks , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[27]  B. Everitt,et al.  Profiles of Cognitive Dysfunction in Chronic Amphetamine and Heroin Abusers , 2000, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[28]  A. Damasio,et al.  Individuals with sociopathic behavior caused by frontal damage fail to respond autonomically to social stimuli , 1990, Behavioural Brain Research.

[29]  J L Lancaster,et al.  Automated Talairach Atlas labels for functional brain mapping , 2000, Human brain mapping.

[30]  Darryl A. Seale,et al.  Sequential Decision Making with Relative Ranks: An Experimental Investigation of the "Secretary Problem"> , 1997 .

[31]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Improved Assessment of Significant Activation in Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI): Use of a Cluster‐Size Threshold , 1995, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[32]  E. Rolls The orbitofrontal cortex and reward. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[33]  T. Robbins,et al.  Choosing between Small, Likely Rewards and Large, Unlikely Rewards Activates Inferior and Orbital Prefrontal Cortex , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  D L Braff,et al.  Use of methods from chaos theory to quantify a fundamental dysfunction in the behavioral organization of schizophrenic patients. , 1996, The American journal of psychiatry.

[35]  Hanspeter Herzel,et al.  Correlations in DNA sequences: The role of protein coding segments , 1997 .

[36]  Martin P. Paulus,et al.  LONG-RANGE INTERACTIONS IN SEQUENCES OF HUMAN BEHAVIOR , 1997 .

[37]  J S Fowler,et al.  Addiction, a disease of compulsion and drive: involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[38]  M. J. Norušis,et al.  SPSS base system user's guide , 1990 .

[39]  F. Moeller,et al.  Impulsivity and history of drug dependence. , 1998, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[40]  Gregory G. Brown,et al.  Prefrontal, Parietal, and Temporal Cortex Networks Underlie Decision-Making in the Presence of Uncertainty , 2001, NeuroImage.

[41]  R W Cox,et al.  AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. , 1996, Computers and biomedical research, an international journal.