Methodological considerations for interpreting the Language Familiarity Effect in talker processing.

The Language Familiarity Effect (LFE)-where listeners are better at processing talker-voice information in their native language than in an unfamiliar language-has received renewed attention in the past 10 years. Numerous studies have sought to probe the underlying causes of this advantage by cleverly manipulating aspects of the stimuli (using phonologically related languages, backwards speech, nonwords) and by examining individual differences across listeners (testing reading ability and pitch perception). Most of these studies find evidence for the importance of phonological information or phonological processing as a supporting mechanism for the LFE. What has not been carefully examined, however, are how other methodological considerations such as task effects and stimulus length can change performance on talker-voice processing tasks. In this review, I provide an overview of the literature on the LFE and examine how methodological decisions affect the presence or absence of the LFE. This article is categorized under: Linguistics > Language in Mind and Brain Psychology > Language.

[1]  A. Fawcett,et al.  Reaction Times and Dyslexia , 1994, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[2]  Adults with Dyslexia Have a Deficit in Voice Recognition , 1994, Perceptual and motor skills.

[3]  D. Pisoni Auditory and phonetic memory codes in the discrimination of consonants and vowels , 1973, Perception & psychophysics.

[4]  S. Pruzansky,et al.  Effects of stimulus content and duration on talker identification. , 1966, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[5]  R. van Hout,et al.  L2 voice recognition: The role of speaker-, listener-, and stimulus-related factors. , 2017, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[6]  R. Schwartz,et al.  The development of language-specific and language-independent talker processing. , 2013, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[7]  Carsten Elbro,et al.  Predicting dyslexia from kindergarten : The importance of distinctness of phonological representations of lexical items , 1998 .

[8]  Susannah V. Levi,et al.  Individual Differences in Learning Talker Categories: The Role of Working Memory , 2015, Phonetica.

[9]  Patrick C. M. Wong,et al.  Learning to recognize speakers of a non-native language: Implications for the functional organization of human auditory cortex , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  U. Goswami,et al.  Phonological awareness deficits in developmental dyslexia and the phonological representations hypothesis. , 1997, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[11]  B. Shaywitz,et al.  A definition of dyslexia , 2003 .

[12]  S. Levi,et al.  Another bilingual advantage? Perception of talker-voice information , 2017, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.

[13]  Ben Maassen,et al.  Identification and discrimination of voicing and place-of-articulation in developmental dyslexia , 2001 .

[14]  Ellen Bialystok,et al.  Words Get in the Way: Linguistic Effects on Talker Discrimination , 2017, Cogn. Sci..

[15]  H. Mitterer,et al.  Neural evidence of allophonic perception in children at risk for dyslexia , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  Mirjam Wester,et al.  Talker discrimination across languages , 2012, Speech Commun..

[17]  S. Goldinger Echoes of echoes? An episodic theory of lexical access. , 1998, Psychological review.

[18]  James M. McQueen,et al.  Phonetic content influences voice discriminability , 2007 .

[19]  Rachel M. Theodore,et al.  Reading ability influences native and non-native voice recognition, even for unimpaired readers. , 2016, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[20]  Anne Cutler,et al.  Abstraction and the (Misnamed) Language Familiarity Effect , 2018, Cogn. Sci..

[21]  B. Pennington,et al.  Deficient implicit phonological representations in children with dyslexia. , 2006, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[22]  Niels O. Schiller,et al.  Different influences of the native language of a listener on speaker recognition , 1997 .

[23]  L. Humes,et al.  The relation between identification and discrimination of vowels in young and elderly listeners. , 1998, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[24]  D. Pisoni,et al.  Talker-specific learning in speech perception , 1998, Perception & psychophysics.

[25]  U. Goswami,et al.  Phonological representations, reading development and dyslexia: towards a cross-linguistic theoretical framework. , 2000, Dyslexia.

[26]  Rachel M. Theodore,et al.  Language exposure facilitates talker learning prior to language comprehension, even in adults , 2015, Cognition.

[27]  R. Bull,et al.  Earwitness identification performance: the effect of language, target, deliberate strategies and indirect measures , 2007 .

[28]  Fernando Cuetos,et al.  Ability for voice recognition is a marker for dyslexia in children. , 2014, Experimental psychology.

[29]  Sarah C. Creel,et al.  Gradient language dominance affects talker learning , 2014, Cognition.

[30]  J. Godfrey,et al.  Performance of dyslexic children on speech perception tests. , 1981, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[31]  Niels O. Schiller,et al.  Evaluation of a foreign speaker in forensic phonetics: A report , 2013 .

[32]  Emily B. Myers,et al.  The impact of musical training and tone language experience on talker identification. , 2015, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[33]  J. Wouters,et al.  Impairments in speech and nonspeech sound categorization in children with dyslexia are driven by temporal processing difficulties. , 2011, Research in developmental disabilities.

[34]  Irwin Pollack,et al.  On the Identification of Speakers by Voice , 1954 .

[35]  Peter D. Bricker,et al.  chapter 9 – Speaker Recognition , 1976 .

[36]  Sarah C. Creel,et al.  Apples and Oranges: Developmental Discontinuities in Spoken-Language Processing? , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[37]  Elizabeth K. Johnson,et al.  Infant ability to tell voices apart rests on language experience. , 2011, Developmental science.

[38]  John D E Gabrieli,et al.  Human Voice Recognition Depends on Language Ability , 2011, Science.

[39]  Niels O. Schiller,et al.  The effect of removing linguistic information upon identifying speakers of a foreign language , 1997 .

[40]  Familiarity, Expertise, and Change Detection: Change Deafness is Worse in Your Native Language , 2014, Perception.

[41]  Bruno L. Giordano,et al.  A language-familiarity effect for speaker discrimination without comprehension , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[42]  Carsten Elbro,et al.  Early linguistic abilities and reading development: A review and a hypothesis , 1996 .

[43]  L. Bahrick,et al.  The development of infant learning about specific face-voice relations. , 2005, Developmental psychology.

[44]  J.P. Eatock,et al.  A quantitative assessment of the relative speaker discriminating properties of phonemes , 1994, Proceedings of ICASSP '94. IEEE International Conference on Acoustics, Speech and Signal Processing.

[45]  J. Démonet,et al.  Perceptual discrimination of speech sounds in developmental dyslexia. , 2001, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[46]  J. Werker,et al.  Speech perception in severely disabled and average reading children. , 1987, Canadian journal of psychology.

[47]  Kirk P. H. Sullivan,et al.  Speaker discrimination in a foreign language: first language environment, second language learners , 2000 .

[48]  Kanae Amino,et al.  Differential effects of the phonemes on identification of previously unknown speakers , 2008 .

[49]  J. Wouters,et al.  Preschool impairments in auditory processing and speech perception uniquely predict future reading problems. , 2011, Research in developmental disabilities.

[50]  L. Verhoeven,et al.  Neural evidence of the allophonic mode of speech perception in adults with dyslexia , 2013, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[51]  J. Gabrieli Dyslexia: A New Synergy Between Education and Cognitive Neuroscience , 2009, Science.

[52]  David Poeppel,et al.  Multiple levels of linguistic and paralinguistic features contribute to voice recognition , 2015, Scientific Reports.

[53]  Ben Maassen, Paul Groenen, Thom Crul, Claire Assma Identification and discrimination of voicing and place-of-articulation in developmental dyslexia , 2001 .

[54]  L. Sprenger-Charolles,et al.  Discrimination of speech sounds by children with dyslexia: comparisons with chronological age and reading level controls. , 2008, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[55]  S. Levi Talker familiarity and spoken word recognition in school-age children* , 2014, Journal of Child Language.

[56]  David B Pisoni,et al.  Identification and discrimination of bilingual talkers across languages. , 2008, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[57]  E. Gerrits,et al.  Early language development of children at familial risk of dyslexia: speech perception and production. , 2009, Journal of communication disorders.

[58]  Harry Hollien,et al.  Perceptual identification of voices under normal, stress and disguise speaking conditions , 1982 .

[59]  Elizabeth K. Johnson,et al.  Effects of language experience and task demands on talker recognition by children and adults. , 2018, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[60]  B. Pennington,et al.  Developmental dyslexia , 2012, The Lancet.

[61]  C. Thompson A language effect in voice identification , 1987 .

[62]  Alvin G. Goldstein,et al.  Recognition memory for accented and unaccented voices , 1981 .

[63]  L. Sprenger-Charolles,et al.  Allophonic mode of speech perception in dyslexia. , 2004, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[64]  I. Pollack,et al.  On the comparison between identification and discrimination tests in speech perception , 1971 .

[65]  G Strube,et al.  The role of language familiarity in voice identification , 1991, Memory & cognition.

[66]  J S Logan,et al.  Cross-language evidence for three factors in speech perception , 1985, Perception & psychophysics.

[67]  Elizabeth K. Johnson,et al.  The Native-Language Benefit for Talker Identification Is Robust in 7.5-Month-Old Infants , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[68]  M. Studdert-Kennedy,et al.  Speech perception deficits in poor readers: auditory processing or phonological coding? , 1997, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[69]  D. Lancker,et al.  Familiar voice recognition: Patterns and parameters. Part I. Recognition of backward voices , 1985 .

[70]  Frank Schlichting,et al.  The imitated voice - a problem for voice line-ups? , 2013 .

[71]  J. Werker,et al.  Phonemic and phonetic factors in adult cross-language speech perception. , 1984, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[72]  A. J. Compton,et al.  Effects of Filtering and Vocal Duration upon the Identification of Speakers, Aurally , 1963 .

[73]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Are speech perception deficits associated with developmental dyslexia? , 1997, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[74]  Talker discrimination in preschool children with and without specific language impairment. , 2013, Journal of communication disorders.

[75]  Heikki Lyytinen,et al.  Speech Perception of Infants With High Familial Risk for Dyslexia Differ at the Age of 6 Months , 2003, Developmental neuropsychology.

[76]  Michelle R. Molis,et al.  Perception of voice and tone onset time continua in children with dyslexia with and without attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. , 2001, Journal of experimental child psychology.