Self-relevance enhances evidence gathering during decision-making.

Despite repeated demonstrations that self-relevant material is prioritized during stimulus appraisal, a number of unresolved issues remain. In particular, it is unclear if self-relevance facilitates task performance when stimuli are encountered under challenging processing conditions. To explore this issue, using a backward masking procedure, here participants were required to report if briefly presented objects (pencils and pens) had previously been assigned to the self or a best friend (i.e., object-ownership task). The results yielded a standard self-ownership effect, such that responses were faster and more accurate to self-owned (vs. friend-owned) objects. In addition, a drift diffusion model analysis indicated that this effect was underpinned by a stimulus bias. Specifically, evidence was accumulated more rapidly from self-owned compared to friend-owned stimuli. These findings further elucidate the extent and origin of self-prioritization during decisional processing.

[1]  A. Voss,et al.  Diffusion models in experimental psychology: a practical introduction. , 2013, Experimental psychology.

[2]  G. Humphreys,et al.  The ubiquitous self: what the properties of self‐bias tell us about the self , 2017, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[3]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Expanding and Retracting From the Self: Gains and Costs in Switching Self-Associations , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[4]  C. Spence,et al.  Self-prioritization in vision, audition, and touch , 2016, Experimental Brain Research.

[5]  J. Pratt,et al.  I before U: Temporal order judgements reveal bias for self-owned objects , 2019, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[6]  C. Macrae,et al.  Good Me Bad Me: Prioritization of the Good-Self During Perceptual Decision-Making , 2019, Collabra: Psychology.

[7]  Gregory L. Wade,et al.  Target self-relevance speeds visual search responses but does not improve search efficiency , 2018, Visual Cognition.

[8]  N. Brady,et al.  Self-face recognition is characterized by “bilateral gain” and by faster, more accurate performance which persists when faces are inverted , 2010, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[9]  G. Humphreys,et al.  The automatic and the expected self: separating self- and familiarity biases effects by manipulating stimulus probability , 2014, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[10]  David J. Turk,et al.  Yours or mine? Ownership and memory , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.

[11]  Marius Golubickis,et al.  How prioritized is self-prioritization during stimulus processing? , 2019, Visual Cognition.

[12]  Hideki Ohira,et al.  An ERP study on self-relevant object recognition , 2007, Brain and Cognition.

[13]  Tom Beckers,et al.  A Bayesian hierarchical diffusion model decomposition of performance in Approach–Avoidance Tasks , 2014, Cognition & emotion.

[14]  M. Conway,et al.  The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. , 2000, Psychological review.

[15]  Thomas V. Wiecki,et al.  HDDM: Hierarchical Bayesian estimation of the Drift-Diffusion Model in Python , 2013, Front. Neuroinform..

[16]  G. Humphreys,et al.  The Integrative Self: How Self-Reference Integrates Perception and Memory , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[17]  Felicia Pratto,et al.  Individual construct accessibility and perceptual selection , 1986 .

[18]  Joshua J. Tremel,et al.  Prior probability and feature predictability interactively bias perceptual decisions , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[19]  William A. Cunningham,et al.  Self-prioritization and perceptual matching: The effects of temporal construal , 2017, Memory & Cognition.

[20]  G. Knoblich,et al.  Self-prioritization of fully unfamiliar stimuli , 2019, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[21]  B. Hommel Event files: feature binding in and across perception and action , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[22]  Antao Chen,et al.  Automatic Prioritization of Self-Referential Stimuli in Working Memory , 2019, Psychological science.

[23]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Attentional control and the self: The Self-Attention Network (SAN) , 2016, Cognitive neuroscience.

[24]  William A. Cunningham,et al.  Self-Relevance Prioritizes Access to Visual Awareness , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  William A. Cunningham,et al.  Exploring the Self-Ownership Effect: Separating Stimulus and Response Biases , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[26]  A. Bayliss,et al.  Grasping the concept of personal property , 2011, Cognition.

[27]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  Diffusion Decision Model: Current Issues and History , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[28]  A. Voss,et al.  Interpreting the parameters of the diffusion model: An empirical validation , 2004, Memory & cognition.

[29]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  Measuring psychometric functions with the diffusion model. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  J. L. Pierce,et al.  The State of Psychological Ownership: Integrating and Extending a Century of Research , 2003 .

[31]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  Individual Differences and Fitting Methods for the Two-Choice Diffusion Model of Decision Making. , 2015, Decision.

[32]  O. Lipp,et al.  Object ownership and action: the influence of social context and choice on the physical manipulation of personal property , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  What cognitive processes drive response biases? A diffusion model analysis , 2011, Judgment and Decision Making.

[34]  C. Sedikides,et al.  Self-Enhancement: Food for Thought , 2008, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[35]  D. A. Kenny,et al.  Treating stimuli as a random factor in social psychology: a new and comprehensive solution to a pervasive but largely ignored problem. , 2012, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[36]  David J. Turk,et al.  Mine and Me: Exploring the Neural Basis of Object Ownership , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[37]  G. Humphreys,et al.  The central locus of self-prioritisation , 2018, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[38]  C. White,et al.  Decomposing bias in different types of simple decisions. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[39]  Blair T. Johnson,et al.  The self-reference effect in memory: a meta-analysis. , 1997, Psychological bulletin.

[40]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Bias in the Brain: A Diffusion Model Analysis of Prior Probability and Potential Payoff , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  R. Todd,et al.  I Saw Mine First: A Prior-Entry Effect for Newly Acquired Ownership , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[42]  C. Macrae,et al.  Self-relevance enhances the benefits of attention on perception , 2018, Visual Cognition.

[43]  M. Lee,et al.  Hierarchical diffusion models for two-choice response times. , 2011, Psychological methods.

[44]  Andreas Voss,et al.  How many trials are required for parameter estimation in diffusion modeling? A comparison of different optimization criteria , 2016, Behavior Research Methods.

[45]  William M. Kelley,et al.  CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE What the Social Brain Sciences Can Tell Us About the Self , 2022 .

[46]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Perceptual effects of social salience: evidence from self-prioritization effects on perceptual matching. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[47]  Tom Verguts,et al.  Predictive information speeds up visual awareness in an individuation task by modulating threshold setting, not processing efficiency , 2016, Vision Research.

[48]  C. White,et al.  Perceptual Criteria in the Human Brain , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  William A. Cunningham,et al.  Parts of me: Identity-relevance moderates self-prioritization , 2019, Consciousness and Cognition.

[50]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Super-capacity me! Super-capacity and violations of race independence for self- but not for reward-associated stimuli. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[51]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The physics of optimal decision making: a formal analysis of models of performance in two-alternative forced-choice tasks. , 2006, Psychological review.

[52]  Theodore Alexopoulos,et al.  I, me, mine: Automatic attentional capture by self-related stimuli , 2012 .

[53]  T. B. Rogers,et al.  Encoding of Personal Information: Self-Other Differences. , 1979 .

[54]  P. Rotshtein,et al.  Self-prioritization and the attentional systems. , 2019, Current opinion in psychology.

[55]  William A. Cunningham,et al.  Mine or mother’s? Exploring the self-ownership effect across cultures , 2018, Culture and Brain.

[56]  Matthew D. Weaver,et al.  Social salience does not transfer to oculomotor visual search , 2015 .

[57]  K. Shapiro,et al.  Personal names and the attentional blink: a visual "cocktail party" effect. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[58]  D. Wentura,et al.  Self-Prioritization Beyond Perception. , 2015, Experimental psychology.

[59]  Bertram Gawronski,et al.  When possessions become part of the self: Ownership and implicit self-object linking , 2016 .

[60]  Wieske van Zoest,et al.  Testing the idea of privileged awareness of self-relevant information. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[61]  R. Chakravarthi,et al.  Does self-prioritization affect perceptual processes? , 2017 .

[62]  B. Hommel Representing Oneself and Others , 2018, Experimental psychology.

[63]  J. Pratt,et al.  It is not in the details: Self-related shapes are rapidly classified but their features are not better remembered , 2019, Memory & cognition.