Redirecting Walking and Driving for Natural Navigation in Immersive Virtual Environments

Walking is the most natural form of locomotion for humans, and real walking interfaces have demonstrated their benefits for several navigation tasks. With recently proposed redirection techniques it becomes possible to overcome space limitations as imposed by tracking sensors or laboratory setups, and, theoretically, it is now possible to walk through arbitrarily large virtual environments. However, walking as sole locomotion technique has drawbacks, in particular, for long distances, such that even in the real world we tend to support walking with passive or active transportation for longer-distance travel. In this article we show that concepts from the field of redirected walking can be applied to movements with transportation devices. We conducted psychophysical experiments to determine perceptual detection thresholds for redirected driving, and set these in relation to results from redirected walking. We show that redirected walking-and-driving approaches can easily be realized in immersive virtual reality laboratories, e. g., with electric wheelchairs, and show that such systems can combine advantages of real walking in confined spaces with benefits of using vehiclebased self-motion for longer-distance travel.

[1]  Jack M. Loomis,et al.  Locomotion Mode Affects the Updating of Objects Encountered During Travel: The Contribution of Vestibular and Proprioceptive Inputs to Path Integration , 1998, Presence.

[2]  Mary C. Whitton,et al.  Walking > walking-in-place > flying, in virtual environments , 1999, SIGGRAPH.

[3]  James Park,et al.  The Brain's Sense of Movement , 2003, The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine.

[4]  R. Klatzky,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Use of Cognitive Versus Perceptual Heading During Imagined Locomotion Depends on the Response Mode , 2022 .

[5]  Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al.  Cognitive factors can influence self-motion perception (vection) in virtual reality , 2006, TAP.

[6]  Doug A. Bowman,et al.  Travel in immersive virtual environments: an evaluation of viewpoint motion control techniques , 1997, Proceedings of IEEE 1997 Annual International Symposium on Virtual Reality.

[7]  Mary C. Whitton,et al.  An evaluation of navigational ability comparing Redirected Free Exploration with Distractors to Walking-in-Place and joystick locomotio interfaces , 2011, 2011 IEEE Virtual Reality Conference.

[8]  Uwe D. Hanebeck,et al.  Motion Compression for Telepresent Walking in Large Target Environments , 2004, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[9]  H. Bülthoff,et al.  Merging the senses into a robust percept , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[10]  Victoria Interrante,et al.  Seven League Boots: A New Metaphor for Augmented Locomotion through Moderately Large Scale Immersive Virtual Environments , 2007, 2007 IEEE Symposium on 3D User Interfaces.

[11]  May Q. Liu,et al.  Muscles that support the body also modulate forward progression during walking. , 2006, Journal of biomechanics.

[12]  Mel Slater,et al.  Using Presence Questionnaires in Reality , 2000, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[13]  F. Bremmer,et al.  Perception of self-motion from visual flow , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[14]  Sabarish V. Babu,et al.  Evaluation of the Cognitive Effects of Travel Technique in Complex Real and Virtual Environments , 2010, IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics.

[15]  Gerd Bruder,et al.  Estimation of Detection Thresholds for Redirected Walking Techniques , 2010, IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics.

[16]  Ingo Fründ,et al.  Inference for psychometric functions in the presence of nonstationary behavior. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[17]  Bernhard E. Riecke,et al.  How to cheat in motion simulation: comparing the engineering and fun ride approach to motion cueing , 2001 .

[18]  Roy A. Ruddle,et al.  The benefits of using a walking interface to navigate virtual environments , 2009, TCHI.

[19]  Uwe D. Hanebeck,et al.  Telepresence Techniques for Controlling Avatar Motion in First Person Games , 2005, INTETAIN.

[20]  Sharif Razzaque,et al.  Redirected Walking , 2001, Eurographics.

[21]  Craig A. Will,et al.  Review of Virtual Environment Interface Technology. , 1996 .

[22]  Betty J. Mohler,et al.  Visual flow influences gait transition speed and preferred walking speed , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  Sharif Razzaque,et al.  Comparing VE locomotion interfaces , 2005, IEEE Proceedings. VR 2005. Virtual Reality, 2005..

[24]  Klaus H. Hinrichs,et al.  Arch-Explore: A natural user interface for immersive architectural walkthroughs , 2009, 2009 IEEE Symposium on 3D User Interfaces.

[25]  Eric Burns,et al.  Combining passive haptics with redirected walking , 2005, ICAT '05.

[26]  S. Klein,et al.  Measuring, estimating, and understanding the psychometric function: A commentary , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[27]  J. Dichgans,et al.  Visual-Vestibular Interaction: Effects on Self-Motion Perception and Postural Control , 1978 .

[28]  Sunjoo K. Advani,et al.  Integrated design of flight simulator motion cueing systems , 2005, The Aeronautical Journal (1968).

[29]  Mel Slater,et al.  Taking steps: the influence of a walking technique on presence in virtual reality , 1995, TCHI.

[30]  Gerd Bruder,et al.  Self-motion illusions in immersive virtual reality environments , 2011, 2011 IEEE Virtual Reality Conference.

[31]  Mark T. Bolas,et al.  Effects of redirection on spatial orientation in real and virtual environments , 2011, 2011 IEEE Symposium on 3D User Interfaces (3DUI).

[32]  M Lappe,et al.  Dynamical use of different sources of information in heading judgments from retinal flow. , 1999, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.