Perceptual decisions regarding object manipulation are selectively impaired in apraxia or when tDCS is applied over the left IPL

This study evaluated whether apraxia can be understood as due to impaired motor representations or motor imagery necessary for appropriate object-use, imitation, and pantomime. The causal role of the left inferior parietal lobe (IPL), which is heavily implicated in apraxia, is also evaluated. These processes are appraised in light of the proposed ventro-dorsal sub-stream of the classic two visual pathway model, where perceptual information from the ventral stream and the dorsal action stream are integrated and essential for object manipulation. Using a task assessing object-use perception, stroke patients with apraxia demonstrated a selective deficit during perceptual decisions reliant on the integration of visible and known object properties to select the appropriate grasp for object-use. This deficit increased with apraxia severity. A dissociation was evident in these patients showing intact non-motoric perceptual decisions regarding the functional semantic relationship between two objects in the absence of the actor (e.g. how a hammer hits a nail). Converging evidence was found using a modified version of the same task in a neuromodulation study that directly targeted the left IPL in healthy participants using transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Application of inhibitory stimulation over the left IPL reduced performance during perceptual decisions regarding object manipulation whilst performance was unaffected during functional semantic decisions. Excitatory stimulation of the left IPL did not affect performance in either task. Combined, these results suggest that the left inferior parietal lobe is critical for motor imagery, and that apraxia may be caused by an inability to use internal motor representations of object manipulation. These results are discussed in terms of motoric and non-motoric perceptual processes and the proposal of an additional ventro-dorsal sub-stream within the dorsal and ventral visual pathways model.

[1]  M. L. Ralph,et al.  Different roles of lateral anterior temporal lobe and inferior parietal lobule in coding function and manipulation tool knowledge: Evidence from an rTMS study , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  S. Frey What Puts the How in Where? Tool Use and the Divided Visual Streams Hypothesis , 2007, Cortex.

[3]  Guy Vingerhoets,et al.  Knowing about tools: Neural correlates of tool familiarity and experience , 2008, NeuroImage.

[4]  L. Buxbaum,et al.  Distinctions between manipulation and function knowledge of objects: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[5]  David P. Carey,et al.  Tapping, grasping and aiming in ideomotor apraxia , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  Murray Grossman,et al.  Left Inferior Parietal Representations for Skilled Hand-Object Interactions: Evidence from Stroke and Corticobasal Degeneration , 2007, Cortex.

[8]  M. L. Lambon Ralph,et al.  The role of conceptual knowledge in object use evidence from semantic dementia. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[9]  R T Knight,et al.  Neural representations of skilled movement. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[10]  Tim Shallice,et al.  The different neural correlates of action and functional knowledge in semantic memory: an FMRI study. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[11]  Ramón Leiguarda,et al.  Limb Apraxia: Cortical or Subcortical , 2001, NeuroImage.

[12]  L. Cohen,et al.  Transcranial direct current stimulation: State of the art 2008 , 2008, Brain Stimulation.

[13]  Magdalena Ietswaart,et al.  Impaired Communication Between the Dorsal and Ventral Stream: Indications from Apraxia , 2016, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[14]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  A distributed left hemisphere network active during planning of everyday tool use skills. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[15]  L. Buxbaum,et al.  Action knowledge, visuomotor activation, and embodiment in the two action systems , 2010, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[16]  G. Vingerhoets Contribution of the posterior parietal cortex in reaching, grasping, and using objects and tools , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[17]  Magdalena Ietswaart,et al.  Memory-driven movements in limb apraxia: is there evidence for impaired communication between the dorsal and the ventral streams? , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  F. Binkofski,et al.  Two action systems in the human brain , 2013, Brain and Language.

[19]  C. Marsden,et al.  The basal ganglia and apraxia. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[20]  Chris Rorden,et al.  Pantomime of tool use depends on integrity of left inferior frontal cortex. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[21]  Vincent Q. Walsh Ethics and Social Risks in Brain Stimulation , 2013, Brain Stimulation.

[22]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[23]  JamesW. Lewis Cortical Networks Related to Human Use of Tools , 2006, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[24]  Angela Sirigu,et al.  A Selective Impairment of Hand Posture for Object Utilization in Apraxia , 1995, Cortex.

[25]  M. Goodale,et al.  The visual brain in action , 1995 .

[26]  Rizzolatti Giacomo,et al.  The Two Dorsal Visual Streams and Their Role in Perception , 2011 .

[27]  R. McIntosh,et al.  Do we have independent visual streams for perception and action? , 2010, Cognitive neuroscience.

[28]  L. Buxbaum Ideomotor Apraxia: a Call to Action , 2001, Neurocase.

[29]  Chris Rorden,et al.  Age-specific CT and MRI templates for spatial normalization , 2012, NeuroImage.

[30]  P. Jackson,et al.  The neural network of motor imagery: An ALE meta-analysis , 2013, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[31]  Gereon R. Fink,et al.  Neural basis of pantomiming the use of visually presented objects , 2004, NeuroImage.

[32]  Dana Samson,et al.  Separating forms of neglect using the Apples Test: validation and functional prediction in chronic and acute stroke. , 2011, Neuropsychology.

[33]  L. Buxbaum,et al.  Neural substrates of knowledge of hand postures for object grasping and functional object use: Evidence from fMRI , 2006, Brain Research.

[34]  M. Brett,et al.  Actions Speak Louder Than Functions: The Importance of Manipulability and Action in Tool Representation , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[35]  Georg Goldenberg,et al.  Imitating gestures and manipulating a mannikin—The representation of the human body in ideomotor apraxia , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[36]  D. Mirman,et al.  Abnormal dynamics of activation of object use information in apraxia: Evidence from eyetracking , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[37]  J G Snodgrass,et al.  Picture naming by young children: norms for name agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1997, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[38]  Jessica Lowell Neural Network , 2001 .

[39]  K. Poeck Ideational apraxia , 2004, Journal of Neurology.

[40]  A. Sirigu,et al.  How we interact with objects: learning from brain lesions , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[41]  Martin Lotze,et al.  Volition and imagery in neurorehabilitation. , 2006, Cognitive and behavioral neurology : official journal of the Society for Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology.

[42]  Carlo Miniussi,et al.  Parietal Lobe Contribution to Mental Rotation Demonstrated with rTMS , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[43]  G. Goldenberg,et al.  Tool use and mechanical problem solving in apraxia , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[44]  Eric Achten,et al.  Tool responsive regions in the posterior parietal cortex: Effect of differences in motor goal and target object during imagined transitive movements , 2009, NeuroImage.

[45]  Mark Hallett,et al.  Transcranial direct current stimulation for the treatment of Parkinson's disease , 2010, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry.

[46]  M. Jeannerod The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[47]  Alan Sunderland,et al.  Tool use and action planning in apraxia , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[48]  Angela R. Laird,et al.  ALE meta-analysis of action observation and imitation in the human brain , 2010, NeuroImage.

[49]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Neural foundations of imagery , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[50]  J. Hermsdörfer,et al.  Grasping tools: Effects of task and apraxia , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[51]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[52]  G. Goldenberg Defective imitation of gestures in patients with damage in the left or right hemispheres. , 1996, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[53]  Will Spijkers,et al.  From pantomime to actual use: How affordances can facilitate actual tool-use , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[54]  M. Koslowsky,et al.  tDCS polarity effects in motor and cognitive domains: a meta-analytical review , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[55]  Walter Paulus,et al.  Transcranial direct current stimulation--update 2011. , 2011, Restorative neurology and neuroscience.

[56]  S. Small,et al.  Fine modulation in network activation during motor execution and motor imagery. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[57]  G. Goldenberg Apraxia and the parietal lobes , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[58]  Chris Rorden,et al.  Improving Lesion-Symptom Mapping , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[59]  M. Brodeur,et al.  The Bank of Standardized Stimuli (BOSS), a New Set of 480 Normative Photos of Objects to Be Used as Visual Stimuli in Cognitive Research , 2010, PloS one.

[60]  Gregory Króliczak,et al.  A common network in the left cerebral hemisphere represents planning of tool use pantomimes and familiar intransitive gestures at the hand-independent level. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[61]  Editorial: Mental practice: clinical and experimental research in imagery and action observation , 2015, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[62]  L. Buxbaum,et al.  Knowledge of object manipulation and object function: dissociations in apraxic and nonapraxic subjects , 2002, Brain and Language.

[63]  Qing Gao,et al.  Evaluation of effective connectivity of motor areas during motor imagery and execution using conditional Granger causality , 2011, NeuroImage.

[64]  V. Walsh,et al.  State-dependency in brain stimulation studies of perception and cognition , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[65]  Kenneth M Heilman,et al.  Postural knowledge of transitive pantomimes and intransitive gestures , 2002, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[66]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Two different streams form the dorsal visual system: anatomy and functions , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[67]  Laurel J. Buxbaum,et al.  Deficient internal models for planning hand–object interactions in apraxia , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[68]  E. Renzi,et al.  Normative Data and Screening Power of a Shortened Version of the Token Test , 1978, Cortex.

[69]  Bradford Z. Mahon,et al.  The Representation of Object-Directed Action and Function Knowledge in the Human Brain. , 2016, Cerebral cortex.

[70]  Scott H. Johnson-Frey Stimulation through simulation? Motor imagery and functional reorganization in hemiplegic stroke patients , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[71]  Alex Martin,et al.  Representation of Manipulable Man-Made Objects in the Dorsal Stream , 2000, NeuroImage.

[72]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Closely overlapping responses to tools and hands in left lateral occipitotemporal cortex. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[73]  M. Nitsche,et al.  Safety criteria for transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) in humans , 2003, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[74]  Julie C. Sedivy,et al.  Impaired access to manipulation features in Apraxia: Evidence from eyetracking and semantic judgment tasks , 2010, Brain and Language.

[75]  Sarah H. Creem-Regehr,et al.  Sensory-motor and cognitive functions of the human posterior parietal cortex involved in manual actions , 2009, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.