Distinguishing self and other in joint action. Evidence from a musical paradigm.

The capacity to distinguish between one's own and others' behavior is a cognitive prerequisite for successful joint action. We employed a musical joint action task to investigate how the brain achieves this distinction. Pianists performed the right-hand part of piano pieces, previously learned bimanually, while the complementary left-hand part either was not executed or was (believed to be) played by a co-performer. This experimental setting served to induce a co-representation of the left-hand part reflecting either the self or the co-performer. Single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation was applied to the right primary motor cortex and motor-evoked potentials (MEPs) were recorded from the resting left forearm. Results show that corticospinal excitability was modulated by whether the representation of the left hand was associated with the self or the other, with the MEP amplitude being low and high, respectively. This result remained unchanged in a separate session where participants could neither see nor hear the other but still infer his presence by means of contextual information. Furthermore, the amplitude of MEPs associated with co-performer presence increased with pianists' self-reported empathy. Thus, the sociality of the context modulates action attribution at the level of the motor control system.

[1]  C. Frith,et al.  Rapid Response Subject Collections , 2022 .

[2]  T. Feinberg,et al.  Where in the brain is the self? , 2005, Consciousness and Cognition.

[3]  M. Hallett,et al.  Involvement of the ipsilateral motor cortex in finger movements of different complexities , 1997, Annals of neurology.

[4]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[5]  Nick Chater,et al.  Perspectives on Imitation , 2004 .

[6]  Jinah Kim,et al.  The Effects of Improvisational Music Therapy on Joint Attention Behaviors in Autistic Children: A Randomized Controlled Study , 2008, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[7]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  The Virtual Co-Actor: The Social Simon Effect does not Rely on Online Feedback from the Other , 2010, Front. Psychology.

[8]  Jeff H. Duyn,et al.  Cortical Systems Associated with Covert Music Rehearsal , 2002, NeuroImage.

[9]  R. Ivry,et al.  Ipsilateral motor cortex activity during unimanual hand movements relates to task complexity. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  Marcel Brass,et al.  The Inhibition of Imitative Response Tendencies , 2001, NeuroImage.

[11]  M. Brass,et al.  Inhibition of imitative behaviour and social cognition , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[12]  P. Haggard,et al.  Self and Other in the Human Motor System , 2006, Current Biology.

[13]  Scott S. Wiltermuth,et al.  Synchrony and Cooperation , 2009, Psychological science.

[14]  Peter E. Keller,et al.  A grammar of action generates predictions in skilled musicians , 2011, Consciousness and Cognition.

[15]  Peter E. Keller,et al.  Joint action in music performance , 2008 .

[16]  S. Blakemore,et al.  Motor activation prior to observation of a predicted movement , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[17]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  Twin Peaks: An ERP Study of Action Planning and Control in Coacting Individuals , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[18]  Matthew Ray,et al.  Seeing vs. believing: Is believing sufficient to activate the processes of response co-representation? , 2007, Human movement science.

[19]  Harold Bekkering,et al.  Fast responders have blinders on: ERP correlates of response inhibition in competition , 2008, Cortex.

[20]  Salvatore Maria Aglioti,et al.  Don't Do It! Cortical Inhibition and Self-attribution during Action Observation , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[21]  Luigi Cattaneo,et al.  One's motor performance predictably modulates the understanding of others' actions through adaptation of premotor visuo-motor neurons. , 2011, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[22]  T. Singer The neuronal basis and ontogeny of empathy and mind reading: Review of literature and implications for future research , 2006, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[23]  J. Poulet,et al.  New insights into corollary discharges mediated by identified neural pathways , 2007, Trends in Neurosciences.

[24]  W. Prinz,et al.  How two share a task: corepresenting stimulus-response mappings. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4-year-old children☆☆☆ , 2010 .

[26]  Istvan Molnar-Szakacs,et al.  Being Together in Time: Musical Experience and the Mirror Neuron System , 2009 .

[27]  Wen-Jui Kuo,et al.  Action Co-representation is Tuned to Other Humans , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[28]  J. Cole,et al.  Enacting Intersubjectivity: A Cognitive and Social Perspective on the Study of Interactions , 2008 .

[29]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Understanding action beyond imitation: reversed compatibility effects of action observation in imitation and joint action. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  Marcel Brass,et al.  Top-down modulation of motor priming by belief about animacy. , 2010, Experimental psychology.

[31]  G. Riva,et al.  Enacting Intersubjectivity: A Cognitive and Social Perspective on the Study of Interactions (Emerging Communication; Studies in New Technologies and Practices in Communication) , 2008 .

[32]  W. Prinz,et al.  Representing others' actions: just like one's own? , 2003, Cognition.

[33]  Cordula Vesper,et al.  A minimal architecture for joint action , 2010, Neural Networks.

[34]  Mark H. Davis,et al.  A Multidimensional Approach to Individual Differences in Empathy , 1980 .

[35]  H. Bekkering,et al.  The mirror neuron system is more active during complementary compared with imitative action , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[36]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  Patrick Haggard,et al.  Supplementary motor area provides an efferent signal for sensory suppression. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[38]  E. Saltzman,et al.  Action Representation of Sound: Audiomotor Recognition Network While Listening to Newly Acquired Actions , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Central cancellation of self-produced tickle sensation , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[40]  Günther Knoblich,et al.  Favouritism in the motor system: social interaction modulates action simulation , 2010, Biology Letters.

[41]  M. Iacoboni,et al.  Getting a grip on other minds: Mirror neurons, intention understanding, and cognitive empathy , 2006, Social Neuroscience.

[42]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Modulation of activity in medial frontal and motor cortices during error observation , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[43]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see , 1998, Nature.

[44]  E Altenmüller,et al.  Cross‐modal plasticity of the motor cortex while listening to a rehearsed musical piece , 2006, The European journal of neuroscience.

[45]  C. Keysers,et al.  Empathy and the Somatotopic Auditory Mirror System in Humans , 2006, Current Biology.

[46]  P. Haggard,et al.  Action observation and execution: What is shared? , 2008, Social neuroscience.

[47]  Jens Haueisen,et al.  Involuntary Motor Activity in Pianists Evoked by Music Perception , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[48]  N. Sebanz,et al.  Psychological research on joint action: Theory and data , 2011 .

[49]  J. Sommerville,et al.  Shared representations between self and other: a social cognitive neuroscience view , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[50]  T. Paus,et al.  Modulation of cortical excitability during action observation: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study , 2000, Neuroreport.

[51]  Henk Aarts,et al.  When competition merges people's behavior: Interdependency activates shared action representations , 2010 .

[52]  Sukhvinder S. Obhi,et al.  Resonating with Others: The Effects of Self-Construal Type on Motor Cortical Output , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[53]  W. Prinz,et al.  Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. , 2001, Acta psychologica.

[54]  Hans-Jochen Heinze,et al.  Shared networks for auditory and motor processing in professional pianists: Evidence from fMRI conjunction , 2006, NeuroImage.

[55]  Stephen M. Rao,et al.  Motor Sequence Complexity and Performing Hand Produce Differential Patterns of Hemispheric Lateralization , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[56]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[57]  Raymond H. Cuijpers,et al.  Joint Action: Neurocognitive Mechanisms Supporting Human Interaction , 2009, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[58]  A Thron,et al.  Playing piano in the mind--an fMRI study on music imagery and performance in pianists. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[59]  L. Fadiga,et al.  The Motor Somatotopy of Speech Perception , 2009, Current Biology.

[60]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The mirror-neuron system: a Bayesian perspective. , 2007, Neuroreport.

[61]  C. Urgesi,et al.  Simulating the future of actions in the human corticospinal system. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[62]  M. Hallett,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetry of ipsilateral motor cortex activation during unimanual motor tasks: further evidence for motor dominance , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[63]  W. Prinz,et al.  Relationships Between Perception and Action: Current Approaches , 1990 .

[64]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  An internal model for sensorimotor integration. , 1995, Science.

[65]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Joint action: bodies and minds moving together , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[66]  P. Fletcher,et al.  Seeing other minds: attributed mental states influence perception , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[67]  Wen-Jui Kuo,et al.  A common coding framework in self–other interaction: evidence from joint action task , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[68]  Marcel Brass,et al.  Was it me or was it you? How the sense of agency originates from ideomotor learning revealed by fMRI , 2009, NeuroImage.

[69]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: interpretations and misinterpretations , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[70]  Antonino Casile,et al.  When Sounds Become Actions: Higher-order Representation of Newly Learned Action Sounds in the Human Motor System , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[71]  S. Koelsch,et al.  Music in the treatment of affective disorders: An exploratory investigation of a new method for music-therapeutic research , 2010 .

[72]  F. Baldissera,et al.  Cyclic time course of motor excitability modulation during the observation of a cyclic hand movement , 2005, Brain Research.

[73]  B. Repp,et al.  Pianists duet better when they play with themselves: On the possible role of action simulation in synchronization , 2007, Consciousness and Cognition.

[74]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  Please Scroll down for Article Social Neuroscience Action Co-representation: the Joint Snarc Effect Action Co-representation: the Joint Snarc Effect , 2022 .

[75]  Paola Scifo,et al.  Understanding Others' Regret: A fMRI Study , 2009, PloS one.

[76]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Evidence for a distributed hierarchy of action representation in the brain. , 2007, Human movement science.

[77]  K. Sugawara,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetry of ipsilateral motor cortex activation in motor skill learning , 2013, Neuroreport.

[78]  W. Prinz A common-coding approach to perception and action , 1990 .

[79]  M. Masson,et al.  Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[80]  Zoubin Ghahramani,et al.  Computational principles of movement neuroscience , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[81]  Scott S. Wiltermuth,et al.  Synchrony and cooperation. Psychol Sci , 2009 .