Lexicalization Patterns and the World-to-Words Mapping

Knowledge of words and knowledge of the world must somehow be linked. Words evoke knowledge about the world, and thoughts about the world are conveyed through words. The precise nature of this linkage is far from known, however. Our goal in this chapter is to shed light on the connection. In the first part of the chapter, we consider arguments for a tight mapping between words and conceptual representations and discuss reasons why these arguments are not entirely convincing. We also briefly consider and dismiss the extreme alternative that there is only the loosest relation between words and conceptual representations of the world. In the second part of the chapter, we turn our attention to a third alternative that we call a “constrained but flexible” mapping. In this section we review data indicating that in at least some domains, the mapping between words and conceptual representations is not tight. We consider the ways in which the mappings may nevertheless be constrained, and we discuss where flexibility is possible despite the constraints. We present data from two studies on the naming of human locomotion that test the ideas about both where the mapping is constrained and where flexibility may enter the picture. In a final section, we discuss implications of the “constrained but flexible” idea for several associated issues. THE TIGHT AND LOOSE MAPPING POSSIBILITIES

[1]  Dedre Gentner,et al.  Why we’re so smart , 2003 .

[2]  Dan I. Slobin,et al.  Language acquisition and conceptual development: Form–function relations: how do children find out what they are? , 2001 .

[3]  Barbara Landau,et al.  Spatial language and spatial representation: a cross-linguistic comparison , 2001, Cognition.

[4]  D. Gentner The development of relational category knowledge , 2005 .

[5]  M. Bowerman Learning how to structure space for language: A crosslinguistic perspective , 1996 .

[6]  David Crystal,et al.  The Cambridge Encyclopedia of Language , 2012, Modern Language Review.

[7]  Kathy Hirsh-Pasek,et al.  Young children can extend motion verbs to point-light displays. , 2002, Developmental psychology.

[8]  Ellen M. Markman,et al.  Constraints Children Place on Word Meanings , 1990, Cogn. Sci..

[9]  Christina Behme,et al.  Language Universals , 2011 .

[10]  Rochel Gelman,et al.  Preschoolers' use of superordinate relations in classification and language , 1986 .

[11]  P. Kay,et al.  Color naming reflects optimal partitions of color space , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[12]  Richard B. Dasher,et al.  Regularity in Semantic Change , 2002 .

[13]  Anna Wierzbicka,et al.  Talking about emotions : semantics, culture, and cognition , 1992 .

[14]  Mutsumi Imai,et al.  Re-evaluating linguistic relativity: Language-specific categories and the role of universal ontological knowledge in the construal of individuation , 2003 .

[15]  Mutsumi Imai,et al.  Transitivity and the Incorporation of Ground Information in Japanese Path Verbs , 1997 .

[16]  S. Carey The child as word learner , 1978 .

[17]  M. Bowerman,et al.  Learning to express motion events in English and Korean: The influence of language-specific lexicalization patterns , 1991, Cognition.

[18]  Steven A. Sloman,et al.  Speaking versus thinking about objects and actions , 2003 .

[19]  Janet W. D. Dougherty,et al.  Salience and relativity in classification , 1978 .

[20]  Rudi Keller,et al.  On Language Change: The Invisible Hand in Language , 1994 .

[21]  Silvia P. Gennari,et al.  Talking About Walking , 2008, Psychological science.

[22]  Mutsumi Imai,et al.  A Cross-Linguistic Comparison of Novel Noun and Verb Learning in English-, Japanese-, and Chinese- Speaking Children , 2010 .

[23]  P. Kay,et al.  The linguistic significance of the meanings of basic color terms , 1978 .

[24]  Steven A. Sloman,et al.  Artifact categorization: The good, the bad, and the ugly , 2007 .

[25]  Péter Szigetvári,et al.  What and When? , 2019, Inauguration and Liturgical Kingship in the Long Twelfth Century.

[26]  Silvia P. Gennari,et al.  Motion events in language and cognition , 2002, Cognition.

[27]  M. Imai,et al.  Scope of linguistic influence: does a classifier system alter object concepts? , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[28]  E. Hunn,et al.  Tzeltal folk zoology : the classification of discontinuities in nature , 1979 .

[29]  D. Slobin From “thought and language” to “thinking for speaking” , 1996 .

[30]  Ellen M. Markman,et al.  Categorization and Naming in Children: Problems of Induction , 1989 .

[31]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  Language and Mind , 1973 .

[32]  Silvia P. Gennari,et al.  Knowing versus Naming: Similarity and the Linguistic Categorization of Artifacts , 1999 .

[33]  Eve V. Clark,et al.  Language and Conceptual Development series How language acquisition builds on cognitive development , 2004 .

[34]  Edward Macneal The Great Eskimo Vocabulary Hoax , 1993 .

[35]  S. Pinker The Stuff of Thought: Language as a Window into Human Nature , 2007 .

[36]  Debi Roberson,et al.  Relatively Speaking: An Account of the Relationship between Language and Thought in the Color Domain , 2010 .

[37]  B. Landau,et al.  “What” and “where” in spatial language and spatial cognition , 1993 .

[38]  P. Kay,et al.  Whorf hypothesis is supported in the right visual field but not the left. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[39]  S. Schiffer Meanings and Concepts , 1999 .

[40]  Mutsumi Imai,et al.  Children's Theories of Word Meaning: The Role of Shape Similarity in Early Acquisition , 1994 .

[41]  E. Spelke,et al.  Ontological categories guide young children's inductions of word meaning: Object terms and substance terms , 1991, Cognition.

[42]  Phillip Wolff,et al.  Evolution and devolution of folkbiological knowledge , 1999, Cognition.

[43]  Marjolyn Verspoor,et al.  Lexical and syntactical constructions and the construction of meaning : proceedings of the bi-annual ICLA meeting in Albuquerque, July 1995 , 1997 .

[44]  J. Carroll,et al.  The function of language classification in behavior , 1958 .

[45]  L. Gleitman,et al.  When English proposes what Greek presupposes: The cross-linguistic encoding of motion events , 2006, Cognition.

[46]  D. Laible,et al.  Constructing Emotional and Relational Understanding: The Role of Affect and Mother-Child Discourse , 2006 .

[47]  P. Kay Basic Color Terms: Their Universality and Evolution , 1969 .

[48]  S. Levinson From outer to inner space: Linguistic categories and non-linguistic thinking , 1997 .

[49]  P. Kay,et al.  Color categories in thought and language: Color naming across languages , 1997 .

[50]  Laura Lakusta,et al.  Spatial language and spatial representation , 2006 .

[51]  A. Majid,et al.  Parts of the body: Cross-linguistic categorisation. Special Issue of: Language Sciences (28(2-3)) , 2006 .

[52]  Kathy Hirsh-Pasek,et al.  Imageability predicts the age of acquisition of verbs in Chinese children* , 2008, Journal of Child Language.

[53]  N A Fox,et al.  Definitions and concepts of emotion regulation. Introduction. , 1994, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[54]  W. Batchelder,et al.  Culture as Consensus: A Theory of Culture and Informant Accuracy , 1986 .

[55]  Eleanor Rosch,et al.  Principles of Categorization , 1978 .

[56]  Dedre Gentner,et al.  Why Nouns Are Learned before Verbs: Linguistic Relativity Versus Natural Partitioning. Technical Report No. 257. , 1982 .

[57]  Dan I. Slobin,et al.  The many ways to search for a frog: Linguistic typology and the expression of motion events , 2004 .

[58]  M. Bennett Empirical Studies of Walking and Running , 1992 .

[59]  K. Nelson Concept, word, and sentence: Interrelations in acquisition and development. , 1974 .

[60]  B. Joseph,et al.  Language History, Language Change, and Language Relationship: An Introduction to Historical and Comparative Linguistics , 1996 .

[61]  T. Au,et al.  Chinese and English counterfactuals: The Sapir-Whorf hypothesis revisited , 1983, Cognition.

[62]  S. Kita,et al.  Sound symbolism facilitates early verb learning , 2008, Cognition.

[63]  Eve V. Clark Learning a Language the Way It Is , 2010 .

[64]  Mario A. Pei,et al.  Language, Thought and Reality: Selected Writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf , 1957 .

[65]  R. McNeill Alexander,et al.  Principles of Animal Locomotion , 2002 .

[66]  J. Lucy,et al.  Language Structure, Lexical Meaning, and Cognition , 2010 .

[67]  Silvia P. Gennari,et al.  Universality and language specificity in object naming , 2003 .

[68]  G. Storms,et al.  Object Naming and Later Lexical Development: From Baby Bottle to Beer Bottle. , 2008 .

[69]  E. Andersen Cups and glasses: learning that boundaries are vague , 1975, Journal of Child Language.

[70]  Susan Carey,et al.  Language acquisition and conceptual development: Whorf versus continuity theorists: bringing data to bear on the debate , 2001 .

[71]  K. Hirsh-Pasek,et al.  Young children associate novel words with complex objects rather than salient parts. , 2007, Developmental psychology.

[72]  Leonard Talmy,et al.  How Language Structures Space , 1983 .

[73]  D. Gentner,et al.  Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Thought , 2003 .

[74]  Anna Papafragou,et al.  Does language guide event perception? Evidence from eye movements , 2008, Cognition.

[75]  G. Murphy,et al.  The Big Book of Concepts , 2002 .

[76]  George Lakoff,et al.  Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things , 1987 .

[77]  L. Gleitman The Structural Sources of Verb Meanings , 2020, Sentence First, Arguments Afterward.

[78]  Mutsumi Imai,et al.  Fast-Mapping and Reorganization: Development of Verb Meanings as a System , 2008 .

[79]  W. H. Warren,et al.  Why change gaits? Dynamics of the walk-run transition. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[80]  D. Crystal The Cambridge Encyclopedia of the English Language , 1998 .

[81]  Miriam Van Staden,et al.  The semantic categories of cutting and breaking events: A crosslinguistic perspective , 2007 .

[82]  Per Stenström,et al.  Introduction to Part 1 , 2007, Trans. High Perform. Embed. Archit. Compil..

[83]  P. Schwanenflugel,et al.  The relationship between category typicality and concept familiarity: Evidence from Spanish- and English-speaking monolinguals , 1986, Memory & cognition.

[84]  D. McDermott LANGUAGE OF THOUGHT , 2012 .

[85]  G. Vigliocco,et al.  Grammatical gender effects on cognition: implications for language learning and language use. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[86]  B. Berlin,et al.  Ethnobiological Classification: Principles of Categorization of Plants and Animals in Traditional Societies. , 1994 .

[87]  Anna Papafragou,et al.  Shake, rattle, ‘n’ roll: the representation of motion in language and cognition , 2002, Cognition.

[88]  P. Kay,et al.  Resolving the question of color naming universals , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[89]  Joan L. Bybee,et al.  Mechanisms of Change in Grammaticization: The Role of Frequency , 2008 .

[90]  J. Devine,et al.  The Versatility of Human Locomotion , 1985 .

[91]  Adrienne Lehrer,et al.  Wine and conversation , 1983 .

[92]  Steven A. Sloman,et al.  Artifacts are not ascribed essences, nor are they treated as belonging to kinds , 2003 .

[93]  Leonard Talmy,et al.  (1) Lexicalization patterns: Semantic structure in lexical forms; and , 1987 .

[94]  S. Levinson,et al.  Rethinking Linguistic Relativity , 1991, Current Anthropology.