Alarms and human behaviour: implications for medical alarms.

Alarms are used in many clinical applications, but they are often less than optimal because the design and implementation of alarms has not always taken the cognitive capacity and processing mechanisms of the user into account. As a result alarms are frequently too loud, irritating, confusing, badly designed, and too numerous, resulting in them often being turned off and hindering, rather than enhancing, task performance. This paper reviews some of the main areas where it is essential to take account of the cognitive system of the user and behavioural processes more generally. Five central areas of concern are discussed: the number of alarms and ways that this might be reduced; false alarm rates and their impact on human responses; the design of alarms and the application of research into auditory cognition on design; intelligent alarm systems; and the proposals for alarm design set out in a recent worldwide medical alarms standard. In each area some background is given and the implications for alarm design and implementation outlined. The conclusion is that there are some indications that alarm design and implementation takes account of relevant research data, but that there is still some way to go before these findings are fully integrated and the situation is improved upon further.

[1]  F. Block We don’t monitor enough , 1986, Journal of Clinical Monitoring.

[2]  W. Hamilton We monitor too much , 1986, Journal of Clinical Monitoring.

[3]  P M Sanderson,et al.  Learnability and discriminability of melodic medical equipment alarms * , 2006, Anaesthesia.

[4]  Michael S. Wogalter,et al.  Handbook of Warnings , 2006 .

[5]  J. Edworthy,et al.  Fewer but better auditory alarms will improve patient safety , 2005, Quality and Safety in Health Care.

[6]  Judy Edworthy,et al.  Designing trend-monitoring sounds for helicopters: methodological issues and an application. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[7]  Marcus Watson,et al.  Sonification Supports Eyes-Free Respiratory Monitoring and Task Time-Sharing , 2004, Hum. Factors.

[8]  Peter Keller,et al.  Meaning from environmental sounds: types of signal-referent relations and their effect on recognizing auditory icons. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[9]  Aldo Cipriano,et al.  An Expert System for Monitor Alarm Integration , 2004, Journal of Clinical Monitoring and Computing.

[10]  Frank E. Block,et al.  A Proposed New Set of Alarm Sounds which Satisfy Standards and Rationale to Encode Source Information , 2004, Journal of Clinical Monitoring and Computing.

[11]  A. Karimi,et al.  Master‟s thesis , 2011 .

[12]  Judy Edworthy,et al.  Acoustic, semantic and phonetic influences in spoken warning signal words , 2003 .

[13]  Pernilla Ulfvengren,et al.  Design of Natural Warning Sounds in Human-Machine Systems , 2003 .

[14]  Bruce N Walker,et al.  Magnitude estimation of conceptual data dimensions for use in sonification. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[15]  Judy Edworthy,et al.  The Perceived Urgency of Speech Warnings: Semantics versus Acoustics , 2002, Hum. Factors.

[16]  Gregory Kramer,et al.  Pitch and loudness interact in auditory displays: can the data get lost in the map? , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[17]  Mariea C. Dunn,et al.  Behavioural implications of alarm mistrust as a function of task workload , 2000, Ergonomics.

[18]  Meera M. Blattner,et al.  A Design for Auditory Display , 2000 .

[19]  J. Wojcicki,et al.  A Novel Approach to the Alarms System for Monitoring of the Patient State in Cardiac Intensive Care Unit , 2000 .

[20]  Gary S. Robinson,et al.  A New Class of Auditory Warning Signals for Complex Systems: Auditory Icons , 1999, Hum. Factors.

[21]  R Graham,et al.  Use of auditory icons as emergency warnings: evaluation within a vehicle collision avoidance application. , 1999, Ergonomics.

[22]  J. Edworthy,et al.  On using psychophysical techniques to achieve urgency mapping in auditory warnings. , 1999, Applied ergonomics.

[23]  S. Barrass,et al.  Using sonification : Special issue on audio and multimedia , 1999 .

[24]  Charles Safran,et al.  Making ICU alarms meaningful: a comparison of traditional vs. trend-based algorithms , 1999, AMIA.

[25]  Stephen Barrass,et al.  Using sonification , 1999, Multimedia Systems.

[26]  Russell L. Martin,et al.  Learning and retention of auditory warnings , 1997 .

[27]  Judy Edworthy,et al.  Designing urgency into auditory warnings using pitch, speed and loudness , 1996 .

[28]  Judy Edworthy,et al.  Warning design : a research prospective , 1996 .

[29]  J P Bliss,et al.  Human probability matching behaviour in response to alarms of varying reliability. , 1995, Ergonomics.

[30]  E C Haas,et al.  Perceived urgency of and response time to multi-tone and frequency-modulated warning signals in broadband noise. , 1995, Ergonomics.

[31]  J. Edworthy,et al.  Are there too many alarms in the intensive care unit? An overview of the problems. , 1995, Journal of advanced nursing.

[32]  J Edworthy,et al.  The design and implementation of non-verbal auditory warnings. , 1994, Applied ergonomics.

[33]  J Edworthy,et al.  Improving Auditory Warning Design: Quantifying and Predicting the Effects of Different Warning Parameters on Perceived Urgency , 1993, Human factors.

[34]  R Hétu,et al.  Audibility and identification of auditory alarms in the operating room and intensive care unit. , 1993, Ergonomics.

[35]  J A Orr,et al.  Intelligent alarms reduce anesthesiologist's response time to critical faults. , 1992, Anesthesiology.

[36]  J Edworthy,et al.  Improving Auditory Warning Design: Relationship between Warning Sound Parameters and Perceived Urgency , 1991, Human factors.

[37]  I. Kestin,et al.  Auditory alarms during anesthesia monitoring. , 1988, Anesthesiology.

[38]  T M O'Carroll,et al.  Survey of alarms in an intensive therapy unit * , 1986, Anaesthesia.

[39]  S I Schmidt,et al.  Alarms: help or hindrance? , 1986, Anesthesiology.

[40]  W. Hamilton Do we monitor enough? We monitor too much. , 1986, Journal of clinical monitoring.

[41]  F. Block Do we monitor enough? We don't monitor enough. , 1986, Journal of clinical monitoring.

[42]  Kerr Jh,et al.  Symposium on anaesthetic equipment. Warning devices. , 1985 .

[43]  Judy Edworthy Interval and Contour in Melody Processing , 1985 .

[44]  R. R. Patterson,et al.  Guidelines for auditory warning systems on civil aircraft , 1982 .

[45]  P. D. Wheeler,et al.  Alarm sounds for medical equipment in intensive care areas and operating theatres , 1981 .

[46]  D Deutsch,et al.  Delayed pitch comparisons and the principle of proximity , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.

[47]  G. A. Miller THE PSYCHOLOGICAL REVIEW THE MAGICAL NUMBER SEVEN, PLUS OR MINUS TWO: SOME LIMITS ON OUR CAPACITY FOR PROCESSING INFORMATION 1 , 1956 .