Repetition of distractor sets improves visual search performance in hemispatial neglect
暂无分享,去创建一个
Árni Kristjánsson | Styrmir Saevarsson | Á. Kristjánsson | H. Hjaltason | Haukur Hjaltason | Sigrúnsif Jóelsdóttir | S. Saevarsson | Sigrúnsif Jóelsdóttir
[1] K. Nakayama,et al. Priming of popout: II. Role of position , 1996 .
[2] J. Driver,et al. New indirect measures of “inattentive” visual grouping in a change-detection task , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.
[3] U. Neisser. Decision-time without reaction-time: Experiments in visual scanning. , 1963 .
[4] H. Müller,et al. Cross-trial priming in visual search for singleton conjunction targets: Role of repeated target and distractor features , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.
[5] M. Corbetta,et al. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[6] K. Nakayama,et al. Priming of pop-out: I. Role of features , 1994, Memory & cognition.
[7] J. Duncan,et al. Visual search and stimulus similarity. , 1989, Psychological review.
[8] Shinsuke Shimojo,et al. Visual surface representation: a critical link between lower-level and higher level vision , 1995 .
[9] M. Mesulam. A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect , 1981, Annals of neurology.
[10] G Gainotti,et al. Mechanisms of unilateral spatial neglect in relation to laterality of cerebral lesions. , 1986, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[11] H. Egeth,et al. Perception without attention: evidence of grouping under conditions of inattention. , 1997, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance.
[12] Michael Erb,et al. Exploring the visual world: The neural substrate of spatial orienting , 2006, NeuroImage.
[13] Christopher Kennard,et al. Visual search and its disorders , 2003, Current opinion in neurology.
[14] A. Treisman. Perceiving and re-perceiving objects. , 1992, The American psychologist.
[15] Howard E. Egeth,et al. Parallel processing of multielement displays , 1972 .
[16] Christian N. L. Olivers,et al. Intertrial priming stemming from ambiguity: A new account of priming in visual search , 2006 .
[17] Chris Rorden,et al. The anatomy of spatial neglect based on voxelwise statistical analysis: a study of 140 patients. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.
[18] M. Jeannerod. Neurophysiological and neuropsychological aspects of spatial neglect. , 1987 .
[19] I. McManus. Charles Dickens: a neglected diagnosis , 2001, The Lancet.
[20] E. Bizzi,et al. The Cognitive Neurosciences , 1996 .
[21] C. Kennard,et al. Impaired spatial working memory across saccades contributes to abnormal search in parietal neglect. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[22] Yuka Sasaki,et al. Processing local signals into global patterns , 2007, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[23] J. Mattingley,et al. Preattentive Filling-in of Visual Surfaces in Parietal Extinction , 1997, Science.
[24] J. Driver,et al. Priming in visual search: Context effects, target repetition effects, and role-reversal effects , 2005 .
[25] J. Mattingley,et al. Parietal neglect and visual awareness , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.
[26] J. P. Cavanagh,et al. The equivalence of target and nontarget processing in visual search , 1971 .
[27] Georg Kerkhoff,et al. Spatial hemineglect in humans , 2001, Progress in Neurobiology.
[28] G. Ettlinger,et al. Unilateral spatial neglect and defective performance in one half of space. , 1985, The International journal of neuroscience.
[29] Ken Nakayama,et al. A primitive memory system for the deployment of transient attention , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.
[30] AÂ rni KristjaÂnsson. Rapid learning in attention shifts : A review , 2005 .
[31] S. Ferber,et al. Revisiting unilateral neglect , 2006, Neuropsychologia.
[32] M. Chun,et al. Contextual Cueing: Implicit Learning and Memory of Visual Context Guides Spatial Attention , 1998, Cognitive Psychology.
[33] J. Driver,et al. Implicit Processing and Learning of Visual Stimuli in Parietal Extinction and Neglect , 2001, Cortex.
[34] C. Kennard,et al. The anatomy of visual neglect. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[35] Á. Kristjánsson. Surface Assignment Modulates Object Formation for Visual Short-Term Memory , 2006 .
[36] K. Nakayama,et al. Priming of pop-out: II. The role of position , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.
[37] J. Driver,et al. Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging and Evoked Potential Correlates of Conscious and Unconscious Vision in Parietal Extinction Patients , 2001, NeuroImage.
[38] Arni Kristjansson,et al. Efficient visual search without top-down or bottom-up guidance , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.
[39] A. Treisman. Perceptual grouping and attention in visual search for features and for objects. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[40] S. Ferber,et al. Spatial awareness is a function of the temporal not the posterior parietal lobe , 2001, Nature.
[41] Á. Kristjánsson. Simultaneous priming along multiple feature dimensions in a visual search task , 2006, Vision Research.
[42] A. Treisman. Perceiving and re-perceiving objects. , 1992, The American psychologist.
[43] B. J. Casey,et al. The Effect of Preceding Context on Inhibition: An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2002, NeuroImage.
[44] C. Kennard,et al. The anatomy of visual neglect , 2003 .
[45] Marlene Behrmann,et al. Hemispatial Neglect and Visual Search: A Large Scale Analysis , 2004, Cortex.
[46] Kenneth M. Heilman,et al. Hemispace and Hemispatial Neglect , 1987 .
[47] DeLiang Wang,et al. The role of priming in conjunctive visual search , 2002, Cognition.
[48] R. Rafal,et al. "Both" means more than "two": localizing and counting in patients with visuospatial neglect , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.
[49] Antonio Torralba,et al. Contextual Priming for Object Detection , 2003, International Journal of Computer Vision.
[50] Pia Rotshtein,et al. On-line attentional selection from competing stimuli in opposite visual fields: effects on human visual cortex and control processes. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.
[51] Árni Kristjánsson,et al. Priming of Color and Position during Visual Search in Unilateral Spatial Neglect , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[52] Á. Kristjánsson. Surface assignment modulates object formation for visual short-term memory. , 2004, Perception.
[53] D. Perani,et al. The anatomy of unilateral neglect after right-hemisphere stroke lesions. A clinical/CT-scan correlation study in man , 1986, Neuropsychologia.
[54] J. Driver,et al. Perceptual awareness and its loss in unilateral neglect and extinction , 2001, Cognition.
[55] Shaul Hochstein,et al. Searching with Unilateral Neglect , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[56] A. Treisman,et al. A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.
[57] B. J. Casey,et al. A developmental fMRI study of attentional conflict , 2001, NeuroImage.
[58] Gazzaniga,et al. 29 Short-Term Memory for the Rapid Deployment of Visual Attention , 2004 .
[59] Árni Kristjánsson,et al. Rapid learning in attention shifts: A review , 2006 .
[60] E. Macaluso,et al. Neural basis for priming of pop-out during visual search revealed with fMRI. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.
[61] M. Albert. A simple test of visual neglect , 1973, Neurology.
[62] C. Olivers,et al. On the dissociation between compound and present/absent tasks in visual search: Intertrial priming is ambiguity driven , 2006 .
[63] P Vuilleumier,et al. Neural response to emotional faces with and without awareness: event-related fMRI in a parietal patient with visual extinction and spatial neglect , 2002, Neuropsychologia.