Movement speed effects on limb position drift

Previous research has shown that even when limb position drifts considerably during continuous blind performance, the topological and metrical properties of generated hand paths remain remarkably invariant. We tested two possible accounts of this intriguing effect. According to one hypothesis, position drift is due to degradation of limb-position information. This hypothesis predicted that drift of static hand positions at movement reversals should not depend on movement speed. According to the other hypothesis, position drift is due to degradation of movement information. This hypothesis predicted that drift of static hand positions at movement reversals should vary with movement speed. We tested these two hypotheses by varying the required movement speed when normal human adults performed back-and-forth manual positioning movements in the absence of visual feedback. Movement distance and direction were well preserved even though hand positions between movements drifted considerably. In accord with the movement error hypothesis, but not in accord with the position hypothesis, the rate at which hand positions drifted depended on movement speed. The data are consistent with the idea that hand position, which defines the origin of the trajectory control coordinate system, and movement trajectory are controlled by distinct neural mechanisms.

[1]  Stephan P. Swinnen,et al.  Proprioceptive control of multijoint movement: bimanual circle drawing , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  S. Freedman The Neuropsychology of spatially oriented behavior , 1968 .

[3]  James Gordon,et al.  Organization of voluntary movement , 1991, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[4]  J. Gordon,et al.  Learning a visuomotor transformation in a local area of work space produces directional biases in other areas. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  J. Kalaska The representation of arm movements in postcentral and parietal cortex. , 1988, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[6]  E A Roy,et al.  The codability of kinesthetic location and distance information. , 1972, Acta psychologica.

[7]  R. Sainburg,et al.  Interlimb transfer of visuomotor rotations: independence of direction and final position information , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  D. Rosenbaum,et al.  Limb position drift: implications for control of posture and movement. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  Apostolos P Georgopoulos,et al.  Neural aspects of cognitive motor control , 2000, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[10]  M. Graziano,et al.  Complex Movements Evoked by Microstimulation of Precentral Cortex , 2002, Neuron.

[11]  J. Gordon,et al.  Accuracy of planar reaching movements , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  J. Lackner,et al.  Coriolis-force-induced trajectory and endpoint deviations in the reaching movements of labyrinthine-defective subjects. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  D. Rosenbaum,et al.  Posture-based motion planning: applications to grasping. , 2001, Psychological review.

[14]  O. Bock,et al.  Goal-directed arm movements in absence of visual guidance: evidence for amplitude rather than position control , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Maintaining internal representations: the role of the human superior parietal lobe , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[16]  J. Wann,et al.  Does limb proprioception drift? , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[17]  Stephan P. Swinnen,et al.  Proprioceptive control of multijoint movement: unimanual circle drawing , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  R. Miall,et al.  Evidence of a limited visuo-motor memory used in programming wrist movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  R. Sainburg,et al.  Differences in control of limb dynamics during dominant and nondominant arm reaching. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  A. Georgopoulos Current issues in directional motor control , 1995, Trends in Neurosciences.

[21]  Anne C. Sittig,et al.  The precision of proprioceptive position sense , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  D. Hoffman,et al.  Muscle and movement representations in the primary motor cortex. , 1999, Science.

[23]  C. C. A. M. Gielen,et al.  The attainment of target position during step-tracking movements despite a shift of initial position , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  C. Ghez,et al.  Trajectory control in targeted force impulses , 1987, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  O. Bock,et al.  Error accumulation and error correction in sequential pointing movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  Michel Desmurget,et al.  Proprioception does not quickly drift during visual occlusion , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[27]  H N Zelaznik,et al.  The role of vision in repetitive circle drawing. , 1996, Acta psychologica.

[28]  J. J. Denier van der Gon,et al.  Integration of proprioceptive and visual position-information , 1999 .

[29]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[30]  D. Rosenbaum Human movement initiation: specification of arm, direction, and extent. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[31]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Cortical mechanisms related to the direction of two-dimensional arm movements: relations in parietal area 5 and comparison with motor cortex , 1983, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  M. Latash,et al.  Effects of altering initial position on movement direction and extent. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  Otmar Bock,et al.  Control of arm movements in a 2-dimensional pointing task , 1990, Behavioural Brain Research.

[34]  J. Paillard,et al.  Active and passive movements in the calibration of position sense , 1968 .

[35]  M. Smyth,et al.  4 – Memory for Movements , 1984 .

[36]  M. Graziano Where is my arm? The relative role of vision and proprioception in the neuronal representation of limb position. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[37]  A. G. Feldman Once More on the Equilibrium-Point Hypothesis (λ Model) for Motor Control , 1986 .

[38]  E. Brenner,et al.  Endpoints of arm movements to visual targets , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  J. Lackner,et al.  Motor adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of reaching movements: endpoint but not trajectory adaptation transfers to the nonexposed arm. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  J. Lackner,et al.  Rapid adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of arm trajectory. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.