The plasticity of intellectual development: insights from preventive intervention.

Debates regarding the plasticity of intelligence are often fired by a confusion between 2 distinct realms of development, that is, between developmental functions (e.g., a group's average IQ over time) and individual differences (e.g., the relative rank ordering of individual IQs within a group). Questions concerning the stability of these 2 realms are statistically independent. Thus there are 2 kinds of intellectual plasticity, and there may be no developmental convergences between them. In the present study, data from an early intervention program were used to investigate the 2 kinds of plasticity separately and to examine certain possible convergences between them. The program involved children at risk for developmental retardation who were randomly assigned at birth to 2 rearing conditions (i.e., educational daycare vs. no educational intervention) and whose intellectual development was then studied longitudinally to 4 years of age. Our findings indicate that developmental functions are moderately alterable through systemic early education, particularly after infancy, whereas individual differences are moderately stable, again particularly after infancy. They also indicate that the 2 kinds of plasticity are independent; the alteration of developmental functions through daycare affects neither the stability nor the determinants of individual differences. We discuss the implications that these findings have for current models of mental development, for the nature-nurture debate, and for arguments concerning the efficacy of early intervention programs.

[1]  C. Ramey,et al.  Preschool compensatory education and the modifiability of intelligence: A critical review. , 1985 .

[2]  R. Mccall Nature–nurture and the two realms of development: A proposed integration with respect to mental development. , 1981 .

[3]  C. Ramey,et al.  Maternal IQ and home environment as determinants of early childhood intellectual competence: a developmental analysis , 1983 .

[4]  K. Borman The Social Life of Children in a Changing Society , 1984 .

[5]  C. Ramey,et al.  The Predictive Power of the Bayley Scales of Infant Development and the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Test in a Relatively Constant Environment. , 1973 .

[6]  William Kessen,et al.  History, theory, and methods , 1983 .

[7]  N. Bayley Manual for the Bayley Scales of Infant Development , 1969 .

[8]  Mark Appelbaum,et al.  Bias in the Analysis of Repeated-Measures Designs: Some Alternative Approaches. , 1973 .

[9]  C. Ramey,et al.  Predicting school failure from information available at birth. , 1978, American journal of mental deficiency.

[10]  R. Mccall Transitions in Early Mental Development. , 1977 .

[11]  P. Mussen,et al.  Carmichael's Manual of Child Psychology , 1972 .

[12]  A. Jensen,et al.  How Much Can We Boost IQ and Scholastic Achievement , 1969 .

[13]  H. E. Jones,et al.  Development of Mental Abilities , 1958 .

[14]  C. Waddington The strategy of the genes , 1957 .

[15]  R. Mccall Challenges to a science of developmental psychology. , 1977 .

[16]  Benjamin S. Bloom,et al.  Stability and change in human characteristics , 1966 .

[17]  C. Ramey,et al.  Infants' home environments: a comparison of high-risk families and families from the general population. , 1975, American journal of mental deficiency.

[18]  A. Jensen,et al.  Bias in Mental Testing , 1981 .

[19]  J. Hunt,et al.  Intelligence and experience , 1961 .

[20]  G. Glass Primary, Secondary, and Meta-Analysis of Research1 , 1976 .

[21]  A. D. B. Clarke,et al.  Early Experience: Myth and Evidence , 1976 .

[22]  Richard B. Darlington,et al.  Radicals And Squares: Statistical Methods For The Behavioral Sciences , 1975 .

[23]  D Cicchetti,et al.  The emergence of developmental psychopathology. , 1984, Child development.

[24]  M. Blank Teaching Learning in the Preschool a Dialogue Approach , 1983 .

[25]  J. Osofsky Handbook of infant development , 1979 .

[26]  Jacob Cohen,et al.  Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences , 1979 .

[27]  L. Cronbach The two disciplines of scientific psychology. , 1957 .

[28]  R. Darlington,et al.  Lasting Effects of Early Education: A Report from the Consortium for Longitudinal Studies. , 1982 .

[29]  R. Bradley,et al.  Home environment and cognitive development in the first 2 years: A cross-lagged panel analysis , 1979 .

[30]  J. Sparling Learningames for the First Three Years: A Guide to Parent-Child Play , 1979 .

[31]  Robert I. Jennrich,et al.  An Asymptotic χ2 Test for the Equality of Two Correlation Matrices , 1970 .

[32]  C. Ramey,et al.  Assessing the intellectual consequences of early intervention with high-risk infants. , 1977 .

[33]  Michael A. Wallach,et al.  Teaching all children to read , 1976 .

[34]  C. Ramey,et al.  Primary prevention of developmental retardation during infancy. , 1982, Prevention in human services.

[35]  M. A. Merrill,et al.  Stanford-Binet intelligence scale : manual for the third revision form L-M , 1973 .

[36]  R. Bradley,et al.  The relation of infants' home environments to mental test performance from six to thirty-six months: A longitudinal analysis. , 1975 .

[37]  Craig T. Ramey,et al.  The modification of intelligence through early experience , 1981 .