Distinguishing cognitive effort and working memory load using scale-invariance and alpha suppression in EEG

Despite being intuitive, cognitive effort has proven difficult to define quantitatively. Here, we proposed to study cognitive effort by investigating the degree to which the brain deviates from its default state, where brain activity is scale-invariant. Specifically, we measured such deviations by examining changes in scale-invariance of brain activity as a function of task difficulty and posited suppression of scale-invariance as a proxy for exertion of cognitive effort. While there is some fMRI evidence supporting this proposition, EEG investigations on the matter are scant, despite the EEG signal being more suitable for analysis of scale invariance (i.e., having a much broader frequency range). In the current study we validated the correspondence between scale-invariance (H) of cortical activity recorded by EEG and task load during two working memory (WM) experiments with varying set sizes. Then, we used this neural signature to disentangle cognitive effort from the number of items stored in WM within participants. Our results showed monotonic decreases in H with increased set size, even after set size exceeded WM capacity. This behavior of H contrasted with behavioral performance and an oscillatory indicator of WM load (i.e., alpha-band desynchronization), both of which showed a plateau at difficulty levels surpassing WM capacity. This is the first reported evidence for the suppression of scale-invariance in EEG due to task difficulty, and our work suggests that H suppression may be used to quantify changes in cognitive effort even when working memory load is at maximum capacity.

[1]  Edward Awh,et al.  The role of alpha oscillations in spatial attention: limited evidence for a suppression account. , 2019, Current opinion in psychology.

[2]  T. Braver,et al.  Cognitive effort: A neuroeconomic approach , 2015, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[3]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  The Capacity of Visual Short-Term Memory is Set Both by Visual Information Load and by Number of Objects , 2004, Psychological science.

[4]  O. Kinouchi,et al.  Optimal dynamical range of excitable networks at criticality , 2006, q-bio/0601037.

[5]  Claude Bédard,et al.  Comparative power spectral analysis of simultaneous elecroencephalographic and magnetoencephalographic recordings in humans suggests non-resistive extracellular media , 2010, Journal of Computational Neuroscience.

[6]  A. Woodward,et al.  Cultural and Developmental Influences on Overt Visual Attention to Videos , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[7]  Oren Shriki,et al.  Optimal Information Representation and Criticality in an Adaptive Sensory Recurrent Neuronal Network , 2016, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[8]  M. Eysenck Anxiety and cognitive-task performance , 1985 .

[9]  Leonardo L. Gollo Coexistence of critical sensitivity and subcritical specificity can yield optimal population coding , 2017, Journal of The Royal Society Interface.

[10]  Woodrow L. Shew,et al.  State-dependent intrinsic predictability of cortical network dynamics , 2015, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[11]  Edward K. Vogel,et al.  Contralateral Delay Activity Tracks Fluctuations in Working Memory Performance , 2018, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[12]  Juliane Britz,et al.  EEG microstate sequences in healthy humans at rest reveal scale-free dynamics , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[13]  L. de Arcangelis,et al.  Self-organized criticality model for brain plasticity. , 2006, Physical review letters.

[14]  M. Esterman,et al.  Models of sustained attention. , 2019, Current opinion in psychology.

[15]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[16]  C. Andersen,et al.  Is Borg’s perceived exertion scale a useful indicator of muscular and cardiovascular load in blue-collar workers with lifting tasks? A cross-sectional workplace study , 2013, European Journal of Applied Physiology.

[17]  Manfred G Kitzbichler,et al.  Cognitive Effort Drives Workspace Configuration of Human Brain Functional Networks , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  Keisuke Fukuda,et al.  Distinct neural mechanisms for spatially lateralized and spatially global visual working memory representations. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[20]  Geoffrey B. Sprinkle,et al.  The effects of monetary incentives on effort and task performance: theories, evidence, and a framework for research , 2002 .

[21]  T. Gisiger Scale invariance in biology: coincidence or footprint of a universal mechanism? , 2001, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.

[22]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Toward a Rational and Mechanistic Account of Mental Effort. , 2017, Annual review of neuroscience.

[23]  E. Bullmore,et al.  Endogenous Human Brain Dynamics Recover Slowly Following Cognitive Effort , 2008, PloS one.

[24]  Jeffrey M. Hausdorff,et al.  Fractal dynamics in physiology: Alterations with disease and aging , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[25]  Maro G. Machizawa,et al.  Neural activity predicts individual differences in visual working memory capacity , 2004, Nature.

[26]  Keisuke Fukuda,et al.  α Power Modulation and Event-Related Slow Wave Provide Dissociable Correlates of Visual Working Memory , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  H. Hotelling Relations Between Two Sets of Variates , 1936 .

[28]  P. Reuter-Lorenz,et al.  Brain connectivity tracks effects of chemotherapy separately from behavioral measures , 2018, NeuroImage: Clinical.

[29]  S. Jaffard,et al.  Wavelet Leaders in Multifractal Analysis , 2006 .

[30]  W. Freeman,et al.  Spatial spectra of scalp EEG and EMG from awake humans , 2003, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[31]  Stephen C. Strother,et al.  The suppression of scale-free fMRI brain dynamics across three different sources of effort: aging, task novelty and task difficulty , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[32]  Edward T. Bullmore,et al.  Broadband Criticality of Human Brain Network Synchronization , 2009, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[33]  Xinwei Deng,et al.  Experimental design , 2012, WIREs Data Mining Knowl. Discov..

[34]  Ellen Garbarino,et al.  Cognitive Effort, Affect, and Choice , 1997 .

[35]  E. Vogel,et al.  Discrete capacity limits in visual working memory , 2010, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[36]  P. Abry,et al.  Bootstrap for Empirical Multifractal Analysis , 2007, IEEE Signal Processing Magazine.

[37]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Motivation and cognitive control: from behavior to neural mechanism. , 2015, Annual review of psychology.

[38]  E. Vogel,et al.  Visual working memory capacity: from psychophysics and neurobiology to individual differences , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[39]  H. Laufs,et al.  Mutual information identifies spurious Hurst phenomena in resting state EEG and fMRI data. , 2018, Physical review. E.

[40]  Biyu J. He Scale-free brain activity: past, present, and future , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[41]  N. Daw,et al.  The opportunity cost of time modulates cognitive effort , 2017, Neuropsychologia.

[42]  Richard P. Heitz,et al.  Effects of incentive on working memory capacity: behavioral and pupillometric data. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[43]  William G. Cochran,et al.  Experimental Designs, 2nd Edition , 1950 .

[44]  Gerhard Werner,et al.  Fractals in the Nervous System: Conceptual Implications for Theoretical Neuroscience , 2009, Front. Physiology.

[45]  Biyu J. He Scale-Free Properties of the Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Signal during Rest and Task , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[46]  F. Paas,et al.  Cognitive Load Measurement as a Means to Advance Cognitive Load Theory , 2003 .

[47]  N. Nicolson,et al.  Perceived Stress and Salivary Cortisol in Daily Life , 1994, Annals of Behavioral Medicine.

[48]  L. Curtin,et al.  The Yerkes-Dodson law. , 1984, Nursing management.

[49]  A. Engel,et al.  Beta-band oscillations—signalling the status quo? , 2010, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[50]  Dante R. Chialvo Critical brain networks , 2004 .

[51]  Edward Awh,et al.  Clear evidence for item limits in visual working memory , 2017, Cognitive Psychology.

[52]  R. Kirk 1 Experimental Design , 2012 .

[53]  Marc W Howard,et al.  Theta and Gamma Oscillations during Encoding Predict Subsequent Recall , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[54]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[55]  Scott Peltier,et al.  Scale‐free brain dynamics under physical and psychological distress: Pre‐treatment effects in women diagnosed with breast cancer , 2015, Human brain mapping.

[56]  D. Plenz,et al.  Neuronal Avalanches in the Resting MEG of the Human Brain , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[57]  Jeffrey N Rouder,et al.  An assessment of fixed-capacity models of visual working memory , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[58]  Minoru Asada,et al.  Information processing in echo state networks at the edge of chaos , 2011, Theory in Biosciences.