Identification of typographically transformed words: instance-based skill acquisition.

Two hypotheses concerning the development of skill at identifying typographically transformed words were tested. One claim is that a general skill independent of specific training instances is involved, and the other is that skill is based on memory for the analysis of specific instances encountered during training. Contrary to the general skill view, a series of experiments demonstrated that transfer of word identification skill was highly specific and occurred only when training and test instances shared common letters printed in the same case (i.e., uppercase or lowercase). Transfer of skill also depended on the visual patterns formed by adjacent letters and word shape. Presentation of a word in training and test phases significantly improved test phase identification of that word even when a unique visual pattern was used. These results are compatible with an instance-based view of word identification skill in which it is assumed that subjects develop skilled analysis of the visual and conceptual characteristics of specific words, and that this skill can be used to identify repeatedly presented words as well as predictable sets of novel words. Language: en

[1]  James L. McClelland,et al.  An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. , 1981 .

[2]  P. Jolicoeur,et al.  Effects of orientation on the identification of simple visual patterns. , 1984, Canadian journal of psychology.

[3]  D. L. Hintzman,et al.  Differential forgetting of prototypes and old instances: Simulation by an exemplar-based classification model , 1980, Memory & cognition.

[4]  Dominic W. Massaro,et al.  Toward a Psychology of Reading , 1978 .

[5]  P. A. Kolers,et al.  Symbol manipulation: Alternatives to the computational view of mind , 1984 .

[6]  L. Squire,et al.  Human memory and amnesia , 1984 .

[7]  Douglas L. Hintzman,et al.  MINERVA 2: A simulation model of human memory , 1984 .

[8]  B. Fischhoff,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory , 1980 .

[9]  H. Kucera,et al.  Computational analysis of present-day American English , 1967 .

[10]  Sandra E. Trehub,et al.  Aging and cognitive processes , 1982 .

[11]  M. Moscovitch A Neuropsychological Approach to Perception and Memory in Normal and Pathological Aging , 1982 .

[12]  Paul A. Kolers,et al.  Memorial Consequences of Automatized Encoding. , 1975 .

[13]  P. A. Kolers,et al.  Procedures of mind. , 1984 .

[14]  Paul A. Kolers,et al.  Specificity of pattern-analyzing skills in reading. , 1978 .

[15]  D. Navon Perception of misoriented words and letter strings. , 1978, Canadian journal of psychology.

[16]  J. Norman,et al.  Reading rotated words. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  A. R. Lurii︠a︡,et al.  The neuropsychology of memory , 1977 .

[18]  Paul A Kolers,et al.  Spatial and ordinal components of form perception and literacy , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[19]  M Coltheart,et al.  Basic processes in reading: computation of abstract letter identities. , 1984, Canadian journal of psychology.

[20]  L. Jacoby,et al.  On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[21]  K R Paap,et al.  Word shape's in poor shape for the race to the lexicon. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[22]  I KOHLER,et al.  Experiments with goggles. , 1962, Scientific American.

[23]  P. A. Kolers,et al.  Graphemic analysis underlying literacy , 1980, Memory & cognition.

[24]  Don L. Scarborough,et al.  Frequency and Repetition Effects in Lexical Memory. , 1977 .

[25]  D W Massaro,et al.  Orthographic regularity, positional frequency, and visual processing of letter strings. , 1977, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[26]  D Milech,et al.  Common and modality-specific processes in the mental lexicon , 1983, Memory & cognition.

[27]  M. Masson,et al.  Interactive processes in sentence comprehension and recognition , 1978, Cognitive Psychology.

[28]  Keith D Horton,et al.  The role of semantic information in reading spatially transformed text , 1985, Cognitive Psychology.

[29]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  When does irregular spelling or pronunciation influence word recognition , 1984 .

[30]  Paul A Kolers,et al.  Specificity of operations in sentence recognition , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[31]  Michael I. Posner,et al.  Abstraction and The Process of Recognition , 1970 .

[32]  E. Rosch,et al.  Cognition and Categorization , 1980 .

[33]  L. Squire,et al.  Preserved learning and retention of pattern-analyzing skill in amnesia: dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. , 1980, Science.

[34]  L. Brooks,et al.  Nonanalytic Cognition: Memory, Perception, and Concept Learning , 1984 .

[35]  John R. Anderson Language, Memory, and Thought , 1976 .

[36]  A Koriat,et al.  What is rotated in mental rotation? , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[37]  Douglas L. Medin,et al.  Context theory of classification learning. , 1978 .

[38]  R M Shiffrin,et al.  Episodic and lexical contributions to the repetition effect in word identification. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[39]  Marilyn Jager Adams,et al.  Models of word recognition , 1979, Cognitive Psychology.

[40]  G. Stratton Vision without inversion of the retinal image. , 1897 .

[41]  R W Schvaneveldt,et al.  An activation--verification model for letter and word recognition: the word-superiority effect. , 1982, Psychological review.

[42]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Strategies and classification learning , 1981 .

[43]  Alexander I. Rudnicky,et al.  Size and case of type as stimuli in reading. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[44]  P. A. Kolers Reading a Year Later. , 1976 .