Peak-End Memory Bias in Laboratory-Induced Dyspnea: A Comparison of Patients With Medically Unexplained Symptoms and Healthy Controls

Objective Memory for unpleasant experiences is dominated by intensity at the experience’s peak and end, with a relative neglect for its duration. Therefore, unpleasant somatic experiences are expected be remembered as less aversive when they end gradually rather than abruptly, even when they last longer (i.e., the “peak-end effect”). We investigated the peak-end effect for dyspnea in healthy participants and in patients with medically unexplained dyspnea (MUD). Methods Two aversive dyspnea-inducing tasks were administered to a clinical MUD sample (n = 29) and a matched healthy control group (n = 29) using a rebreathing paradigm (60-second room air, 150-second rebreathing). In a short trial, the breathing system (mouthpiece) was removed immediately after peak dyspnea. In a long trial, breathing was switched to room air after peak dyspnea and continued in the breathing system for 150 seconds (order was counterbalanced across participants). Respiratory parameters were continuously measured, and dyspnea was rated every 10 seconds. Relative unpleasantness of the dyspneic episode was assessed with forced choice questions. Results More than 70% of the healthy group found the short episode worse than the long one despite equal maximal dyspnea (p = .02). Patients with MUD did not show this peak-end effect (p = .58). The latter had deficient recovery of dyspnea compared with the controls (42.08 [21.86] versus 17.51 [11.18], p < .001), which could not be explained by differences in respiratory physiology. Conclusions The peak-end effect in dyspnea has important implications for dyspnea measurement. Its absence in patients with MUD suggests a critical role of distorted perceptual-cognitive processing of aversive somatic sensations in patients with medically unexplained symptoms.

[1]  R. Monson,et al.  Patients with multiple unexplained symptoms. Their characteristics, functional health, and health care utilization. , 1986, Archives of internal medicine.

[2]  G. Rafferty,et al.  Relation of hypocapnic symptoms to rate of fall of end-tidal PCO2 in normal subjects. , 1994, Respiratory medicine.

[3]  D. Kahneman,et al.  When More Pain Is Preferred to Less: Adding a Better End , 1993 .

[4]  Omer Van den Bergh,et al.  Repeated experiences of air hunger and ventilatory behavior in response to hypercapnia in the standardized rebreathing test: Effects of anxiety , 2008, Biological Psychology.

[5]  T. Hedner,et al.  Pain Recollection after Chest Pain of Cardiac Origin , 2000, Cardiology.

[6]  S. Lareau,et al.  Memory for symptoms in COPD patients: how accurate are their reports? , 2001, The European respiratory journal.

[7]  A. Stone,et al.  Does the peak-end phenomenon observed in laboratory pain studies apply to real-world pain in rheumatoid arthritics? , 2000, The journal of pain : official journal of the American Pain Society.

[8]  G Borg,et al.  Ratings of Perceived Exertion and Heart Rates During Short-Term Cycle Exercise and Their Use in a New Cycling Strength Test* , 1982, International journal of sports medicine.

[9]  M. Decramer,et al.  Distorted symptom perception in patients with medically unexplained symptoms. , 2010, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[10]  O. Van den Bergh,et al.  Psychosomatic symptoms and breathing pattern. , 2000, Journal of psychosomatic research.

[11]  Christiane,et al.  World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. , 2004, Journal international de bioethique = International journal of bioethics.

[12]  H. Logan,et al.  Memory for stress-associated acute pain. , 2004, The journal of pain : official journal of the American Pain Society.

[13]  M. Hotopf,et al.  Medically unexplained symptoms in frequent attenders of secondary health care: retrospective cohort study , 2001, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[14]  J. Houtveen,et al.  Recall bias in reporting medically unexplained symptoms comes from semantic memory. , 2007, Journal of psychosomatic research.

[15]  A. Stone,et al.  Measuring clinical pain in chronic widespread pain: selected methodological issues. , 2003, Best practice & research. Clinical rheumatology.

[16]  Joseph E. Schwartz,et al.  Variability of Momentary Pain Predicts Recall of Weekly Pain: A Consequence of the Peak (or Salience) Memory Heuristic , 2005, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[17]  J. P. Lund,et al.  Memories of chronic pain and perceptions of relief , 1998, Pain.

[18]  David Laibson,et al.  Experienced Utility and Objective Happiness: A Moment-Based Approach , 2001 .

[19]  R Smith,et al.  PubMed Central: creating an Aladdin's cave of ideas , 2001, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[20]  J Clément,et al.  Unsteadiness of breathing in patients with hyperventilation syndrome and anxiety disorders. , 1997, The European respiratory journal.

[21]  Martin A. Safer,et al.  Effects of present pain level on recall of chronic pain and medication use , 1993, Pain.

[22]  Daniel Kahneman,et al.  Memories of colonoscopy: a randomized trial , 2003, Pain.

[23]  Wan Li,et al.  Air hunger and ventilation in response to hypercapnia: Effects of repetition and anxiety , 2006, Physiology & Behavior.

[24]  D. Read,et al.  A clinical method for assessing the ventilatory response to carbon dioxide. , 1967, Australasian annals of medicine.

[25]  J M Kinney,et al.  Effects of respiratory apparatus on breathing pattern. , 1980, Journal of applied physiology: respiratory, environmental and exercise physiology.

[26]  B. Fredrickson Extracting meaning from past affective experiences: The importance of peaks, ends, and specific emotions , 2000 .

[27]  Richard J. Brown,et al.  Psychological mechanisms of medically unexplained symptoms: an integrative conceptual model. , 2004, Psychological bulletin.

[28]  G. Kent,et al.  Memory of dental pain , 1985, Pain.