Chinese character decoding: a semantic bias?

The effects of semantic and phonetic radicals on Chinese character decoding were examined. Our results suggest that semantic and phonetic radicals are each available for access when a corresponding task emphasizes one or the other kind of radical. But in a more neutral lexical recognition task, the semantic radical is more informative. Semantic radicals that correctly pertain to character meaning facilitated reaction time in semantic categorization tasks (Experiment #1), while radicals that had no immediately interpretable relation to character meaning had a strong inhibitory effect. Likewise, phonetic radicals that accurately indicated a character’s pronunciation facilitated a homonym recognition task (Experiment #2), whereas phonetic radicals that differed significantly in pronunciation from their character inhibited homonym recognition. In a lexical decision task (Experiment #3) where each character had either a blurred semantic radical or a blurred phonetic radical, the characters with a blurred semantic radical elicited a significantly higher error rate and a trend for longer response times. These results are interpreted to indicate that while educated native Chinese speakers have full use of both semantic and phonetic paths to character decoding, there is a slight predisposition to semantic decoding strategies over phonetic ones indicating that the semantic path is the default means of character recognition.

[1]  D. Allport,et al.  What Are the Functional Orthographic Units in Chinese Word Recognition: The Stroke or the Stroke Pattern? , 1996 .

[2]  M. Coltheart,et al.  Serial processing in reading aloud: Evidence for dual-route models of reading. , 1994 .

[3]  C. Perfetti,et al.  Reading in English and Chinese: Evidence for a "universal" phonological principle. , 1992 .

[4]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. , 1989, Psychological review.

[5]  Michael Garman,et al.  Psycholinguistics: Accessing the mental lexicon , 1990 .

[6]  L. Feldman,et al.  Semantic radicals contribute to the visual identification of Chinese characters , 1999 .

[7]  Ram Frost,et al.  Orthography, phonology, morphology, and meaning , 1992 .

[8]  M Coltheart,et al.  DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. , 2001, Psychological review.

[9]  Kenneth I Forster,et al.  DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy , 2003, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[10]  M. Snowling,et al.  Subtypes of developmental dyslexia , 1997 .

[11]  Richard C. Anderson,et al.  Role of Radical Awareness in the Character and Word Acquisition of Chinese Children. Technical Report No. 615. , 1995 .

[12]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. , 1996, Psychological review.

[13]  S. Chan The Chinese Learner--A Question of Style. , 1999 .

[14]  Charles A. Perfetti,et al.  The time course of graphic, phonological, and semantic activation in Chinese character identification , 1998 .

[15]  Nancy Pine,et al.  Decoding Strategies Used by Chinese Primary School Children , 2003 .

[16]  William D. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  The relative time course of semantic and phonological activation in reading Chinese. , 2000 .

[17]  W. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  The relative time course of semantic and phonological activation in reading Chinese. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[18]  C. A. Weaver,et al.  Psychology of Reading , 2012 .

[19]  Man-Ying Wang,et al.  From Unit to Gestalt: Perceptual Dynamics in Recognizing Chinese Characters , 1992 .

[20]  N. Chikamatsu,et al.  The effects of L1 orthography on L2 word recognition: A study of American and Chinese learners of Japanese , 1996, Studies in Second Language Acquisition.

[21]  N Osaka,et al.  Size of saccade and fixation duration of eye movements during reading: psychophysics of Japanese text processing. , 1992, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[22]  B. Weekes,et al.  Anomia without dyslexia in Chinese , 1997 .

[23]  Li Hai Tan,et al.  Activation of phonological codes before access to character meaning in written Chinese , 1996 .

[24]  K. Forster,et al.  Masked Phonological Priming in Reading Chinese Words Depends on the Task , 1999 .

[25]  H. Shu,et al.  The subtypes of developmental dyslexia in Chinese: evidence from three cases. , 2005, Dyslexia.

[26]  Marcus Taft,et al.  Morphology, Orthography, and Phonology in Reading Chinese Compound Words , 1999 .

[27]  M. Taft,et al.  The nature of the mental representation of radicals in Chinese: a priming study. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[28]  M. Taft,et al.  Submorphemic Processing in Reading Chinese , 2001 .

[29]  I. Liu,et al.  Cueing Function of Fragments of Chinese Characters in Reading , 1983 .

[30]  Ying Liu,et al.  The lexical constituency model: some implications of research on Chinese for general theories of reading. , 2005, Psychological review.

[31]  Rumjahn Hoosain,et al.  Psycholinguistic Implications for Linguistic Relativity: A Case Study of Chinese , 1991 .