Interpreting Scientific and Engineering Practices: Integrating the cognitive, social, and cultural dimensions

Cognitive studies of science and technology (“cognitive studies”) participate in two interdisciplinary fields: cognitive science and science and technology studies (STS). My analysis starts from issues about how cognitive studies are situated with respect to the social and cultural research programs in STS. As we will see, these issues have implications for how cognitive studies are situated within cognitive science as well. Within STS there is a perceived divide between cognitive accounts and social and cultural (“socio-cultural”) accounts of knowledge construction, evaluation, and transmission. Socio-cultural accounts are dominant, and have tended to claim that cognitive factors are inconsequential to interpreting these practices. Scientists are seen as having interests and motivations and as being members of cultures, but cognition remains, in effect, “black boxed.” Cognitive studies accounts, for their part, have payed deference to the importance of the social and cultural dimensions of practice, but have not, by and large, made these dimensions an integral part of their analysis. The situation has fostered a perception of incompatibility between cognitive and socio-cultural accounts. One clear indication of this perception is the now-expired infamous “ten-year moratorium” on cognitive explanations issued first in 1986 by Bruno Latour and Stephen Woolgar (Latour and Woolgar 1986, p. 280; Latour 1987, p. 247), by which time they claimed, all pertinent aspects of science would be explained in terms of socio-cultural factors. Perceptions to the contrary, any such divide is artificial. Producing scientific knowledge requires the kind of sophisticated cognition that only rich social, cultural, and material environments can enable. Thus, the major challenge for interpreting scientific and engineering knowledge-producing practices is to develop accounts that capture the fusion of the social cognitive cultural dimensions in these. I will argue that the perception stems not from a fundamental incompatibility between cognitive and socio-cultural accounts of science and technology, but rather that integration has been hampered by implicit and explicit notions of ‘cognition’ employed on both sides of the perceived divide. Implicit echoes of Cartesian dualism underlie the anti-cognitive stance in socio-

[1]  Herbert A. Simon,et al.  The Sciences of the Artificial , 1970 .

[2]  Taxonomic changes and the particle-wave debate in early nineteenth-century Britain , 1995 .

[3]  J. Elman,et al.  Rethinking Innateness: A Connectionist Perspective on Development , 1996 .

[4]  Richard Catrambone,et al.  Perceptual Simulation in Analogical Problem Solving , 2002 .

[5]  A. Glenberg,et al.  Comprehension of illustrated text: Pictures help to build mental models☆ , 1992 .

[6]  Stephanie D. Teasley,et al.  Perspectives on socially shared cognition , 1991 .

[7]  Christopher Lawrence,et al.  How scientists explain disease , 2001, Medical History.

[8]  G. Lakoff Women, fire, and dangerous things : what categories reveal about the mind , 1989 .

[9]  Donald A. Norman,et al.  Cognitive artifacts , 1991 .

[10]  B. Latour,et al.  Laboratory Life: The Construction of Scientific Facts , 1979 .

[11]  C. B. Scrignar Traumatic stress: The effects of overwhelming experience on mind, body, and society , 1997 .

[12]  Jiajie Zhang,et al.  The Nature of External Representations in Problem Solving , 1997, Cogn. Sci..

[13]  Herbert A. Simon,et al.  The Processes of Scientific Discovery: The Strategy of Experimentation , 1988, Cogn. Sci..

[14]  Michael B. Usher,et al.  Science in action , 1993, Nature.

[15]  Jacquetta Hill Culture in Mind: Cognition, Culture, and the Problem of Meaning , 1998 .

[16]  M. Gorman,et al.  Interpreting Invention as a Cognitive Process: The Case of Alexander Graham Bell, Thomas Edison, and the Telephone , 1990 .

[17]  A. Jonker Origins of the modern mind. Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition , 1998 .

[18]  N. Nersessian The Cognitive Basis of Science: The cognitive basis of model-based reasoning in science , 2002 .

[19]  O. Braddick,et al.  Seeing in Depth , 2008 .

[20]  Michael Lynch,et al.  Art and Artifact in Laboratory Science: A Study of Shop Work and Shop Talk in a Research Laboratory , 1985 .

[21]  D. Gooding Experiment and the Making of Meaning: Human Agency in Scientific Observation and Experiment , 1990 .

[22]  H. Simon,et al.  Studies of Scientific Discovery: Complementary Approaches and Convergent Findings , 1999 .

[23]  N. Presmeg The body in the mind: The bodily basis of meaning, imagination and reason , 1992 .

[24]  C. Geertz,et al.  The Interpretation of Cultures , 1973 .

[25]  Nancy J. Nersessian,et al.  Faraday to Einstein: Constructing Meaning in Scientific Theories , 1984 .

[26]  R. Tweney Faraday’s Discovery of Induction: A Cognitive Approach , 1985 .

[27]  John Haugeland,et al.  Artificial intelligence - the very idea , 1987 .

[28]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[29]  L. S. Vygotskiĭ,et al.  Mind in society : the development of higher psychological processes , 1978 .

[30]  G. Lakoff Philosophy in the flesh , 1999 .

[31]  Felix Stalder,et al.  Pandora's Hope: Essays on the Reality of Science Studies , 2000, Inf. Soc..

[32]  J. Greeno THE SITUATIVITY OF KNOWING, LEARNING, AND RESEARCH , 1998 .

[33]  J. Lave Cognition in Practice: Outdoors: a social anthropology of cognition in practice , 1988 .

[34]  J. Stevenson The cultural origins of human cognition , 2001 .

[35]  H. Simon,et al.  Scientific discovery as problem solving , 1981, Synthese.

[36]  D. Norman,et al.  A representational analysis of numeration systems , 1995, Cognition.

[37]  Lucy A. Suchman,et al.  Plans and Situated Actions: The Problem of Human-Machine Communication (Learning in Doing: Social, , 1987 .

[38]  S. Kosslyn Image and Brain , 1994 .

[39]  J. Greeno A perspective on thinking. , 1989 .

[40]  G. Davie The Democratic Intellect , 1961 .

[41]  Brian Bowers,et al.  Innovation in Maxwell's Electromagnetic Theory , 1992 .

[42]  Elinor Ochs,et al.  Down to the wire: The cultural clock of physicists and the discourse of consensus , 1997, Language in Society.

[43]  Mauricio Suárez,et al.  Model-Based Reasoning in Scientific Discovery , 1999 .

[44]  Lawrence W. Barsalou,et al.  The role of situations in concept learning , 1996 .

[45]  Andrew Pickering,et al.  The mangle of practice : time, agency, and science , 1997 .

[46]  K. Knorr-Cetina,et al.  Epistemic cultures : how the sciences make knowledge , 1999 .

[47]  Michael E. Gorman,et al.  Mind in the World: Cognition and Practice in the Invention of the Telephone , 1997 .

[48]  Hanne Leth Andersen,et al.  Categorization, anomalies and the discovery of nuclear fission , 1996 .

[49]  H. Rheinberger Toward a History of Epistemic Things: Synthesizing Proteins in the Test Tube , 1997 .

[50]  R. Tweney,et al.  The practice of mathematics and science: From calculus to the clothesline problem , 1998 .

[51]  N. Nersessian Opening the Black Box: Cognitive Science and History of Science , 1995, Osiris.

[52]  L. Barsalou,et al.  Whither structured representation? , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[53]  K. Dunbar HOW SCIENTISTS REALLY REASON: SCIENTIFIC REASONING IN REAL-WORLD LABORATORIES , 1995 .

[54]  J. Greeno Situations, mental models, and generative knowledge. , 1989 .

[55]  Allen Newell,et al.  Human Problem Solving. , 1973 .

[56]  David W. Carraher,et al.  Mathematics in the streets and in schools , 1985 .

[57]  Nancy J. Nersessian,et al.  Faraday’s Field Concept , 1985 .

[58]  R. Tweney Epistemic Artifacts: Michael Faraday’s Search for the Optical Effects of Gold , 2002 .

[59]  Martin Stacey,et al.  Scientific Discovery: Computational Explorations of the Creative Processes , 1988 .

[60]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Cues to food location that domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) of different ages do and do not use , 2000, Animal Cognition.

[61]  R. Nisbett,et al.  Culture and systems of thought: holistic versus analytic cognition. , 2001, Psychological review.