To Do or Not to Do: The Neural Signature of Self-Control
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. Probability effects in the stop-signal paradigm: The insula and the significance of failed inhibition , 2006, Brain Research.
[2] M. Brass,et al. Internally generated and directly cued task sets: an investigation with fMRI , 2005, Neuropsychologia.
[3] Tor D. Wager,et al. Common and unique components of response inhibition revealed by fMRI , 2005, NeuroImage.
[4] Philipp Slusallek,et al. Introduction to real-time ray tracing , 2005, SIGGRAPH Courses.
[5] Henrik Walter,et al. Neural correlates of frustration , 2005, Neuroreport.
[6] Simon B. Eickhoff,et al. A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data , 2005, NeuroImage.
[7] Parashkev Nachev,et al. Volition and Conflict in Human Medial Frontal Cortex , 2005, Current Biology.
[8] G. Aschersleben,et al. Intention-based and stimulus-based mechanisms in action selection , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.
[9] M. Walton,et al. Interactions between decision making and performance monitoring within prefrontal cortex , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.
[10] B. Libet. Mind Time: The Temporal Factor in Consciousness , 2004 .
[11] F. Bermpohl,et al. Cortical midline structures and the self , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[12] R. Passingham,et al. Attention to Intention , 2004, Science.
[13] H. Critchley,et al. Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.
[14] R. Elliott,et al. Response inhibition and impulsivity: an fMRI study , 2003, Neuropsychologia.
[15] Ewald Moser,et al. The preparation and readiness for voluntary movement: a high-field event-related fMRI study of the Bereitschafts-BOLD response , 2003, NeuroImage.
[16] T. Robbins,et al. Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[17] Jordan Grafman,et al. The Roles of Timing and Task Order during Task Switching , 2002, NeuroImage.
[18] J. Bjork,et al. Two models of impulsivity: relationship to personality traits and psychopathology , 2002, Biological Psychiatry.
[19] L. Deecke,et al. The Preparation and Execution of Self-Initiated and Externally-Triggered Movement: A Study of Event-Related fMRI , 2002, NeuroImage.
[20] M. Petrides,et al. Wisconsin Card Sorting Revisited: Distinct Neural Circuits Participating in Different Stages of the Task Identified by Event-Related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[21] E. Bullmore,et al. Mapping Motor Inhibition: Conjunctive Brain Activations across Different Versions of Go/No-Go and Stop Tasks , 2001, NeuroImage.
[22] Robert W. Kentridge. Review: The Feeling of What Happens: Body, Emotion and the Making of Consciousness , 2000 .
[23] R. Passingham,et al. Self-initiated versus externally triggered movements. II. The effect of movement predictability on regional cerebral blood flow. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[24] B. Feige,et al. The Role of Higher-Order Motor Areas in Voluntary Movement as Revealed by High-Resolution EEG and fMRI , 1999, NeuroImage.
[25] Julie J. Exline,et al. Virtue, personality, and social relations: self-control as the moral muscle. , 1999, Journal of personality.
[26] E. Stein,et al. Right hemispheric dominance of inhibitory control: an event-related functional MRI study. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[27] P. Haggard,et al. On the relation between brain potentials and the awareness of voluntary movements , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.
[28] A. Damasio. The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness , 1999 .
[29] M. Jahanshahi,et al. The mode of movement selection Movement-related cortical potentials prior to freely selected and repetitive movements , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.
[30] Y. Miyashita,et al. No‐go dominant brain activity in human inferior prefrontal cortex revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging , 1998, The European journal of neuroscience.
[31] C. Marsden,et al. Self-initiated versus externally triggered movements. I. An investigation using measurement of regional cerebral blood flow with PET and movement-related potentials in normal and Parkinson's disease subjects. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[32] G. McCarthy,et al. Functional organization of human supplementary motor cortex studied by electrical stimulation , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[33] S C Gandevia,et al. Transcranial magnetic stimulation can influence the selection of motor programmes. , 1990, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.
[34] James L. McClelland,et al. On the control of automatic processes: a parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. , 1990, Psychological review.
[35] G. Goldberg. Supplementary motor area structure and function: Review and hypotheses , 1985, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[36] D. Pandya,et al. Supplementary motor area structure and function: Review and hypotheses , 1985 .
[37] G. Logan,et al. On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[38] B. Libet,et al. Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential). The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[39] D. Norman,et al. Attention to Action: Willed and Automatic Control of Behavior Technical Report No. 8006. , 1980 .
[40] D. Norman,et al. Attention to action: Willed and automatic control , 1980 .