Theory meets pigeons: The influence of reward-magnitude on discrimination-learning
暂无分享,去创建一个
Jonas Rose | Onur Güntürkün | Robert Schmidt | R. Schmidt | O. Güntürkün | Jonas Rose | M. Grabemann | Marco Grabemann
[1] W. Schultz. Getting Formal with Dopamine and Reward , 2002, Neuron.
[2] K. Doya,et al. Representation of Action-Specific Reward Values in the Striatum , 2005, Science.
[3] E. Miller,et al. Different time courses of learning-related activity in the prefrontal cortex and striatum , 2005, Nature.
[4] P. Glimcher,et al. Midbrain Dopamine Neurons Encode a Quantitative Reward Prediction Error Signal , 2005, Neuron.
[5] A J Neuringer,et al. Effects of reinforcement magnitude on choice and rate of responding. , 1967, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[6] P. Dayan,et al. A framework for mesencephalic dopamine systems based on predictive Hebbian learning , 1996, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[7] W. Schultz. Multiple reward signals in the brain , 2000, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[8] Tobias Otto,et al. The Biopsychology-Toolbox: A free, open-source Matlab-toolbox for the control of behavioral experiments , 2008, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.
[9] L. Crespi. Quantitative variation of incentive and performance in the white rat. , 1942 .
[10] Richard S. Sutton,et al. Learning to predict by the methods of temporal differences , 1988, Machine Learning.
[11] A G Barto,et al. Toward a modern theory of adaptive networks: expectation and prediction. , 1981, Psychological review.
[12] J. Horvitz,et al. Dopaminergic Mechanisms in Actions and Habits , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[13] H. W. Nissen,et al. The Influence of Amount of Incentive on Delayed Response Performances of Chimpanzees , 1935 .
[14] K. M. Michels. Response latency as a function of the amount of reinforcement , 1957 .
[15] W. Schultz. Multiple dopamine functions at different time courses. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.
[16] J. Wickens,et al. A cellular mechanism of reward-related learning , 2001, Nature.
[17] Reversal learning as a function of the size of the reward during acquisition and reversal. , 1967, Journal of experimental psychology.
[18] N. Guttman. Equal-reinforcement values for sucrose and glucose solutions compared with equal-sweetness values. , 1954, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[19] R. Black. Shifts in magnitude of reward and contrast effects in instrumental and selective learning: a reinterpretation. , 1968, Psychological review.
[20] D. Wickens,et al. Effect of differential quantity of reward on acquisition and performance of a maze habit. , 1954, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[21] K. Doya. Modulators of decision making , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.
[22] A. Graybiel. The basal ganglia: learning new tricks and loving it , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[23] N. Guttman,et al. Operant conditioning, extinction, and periodic reinforcement in relation to concentration of sucrose used as reinforcing agent. , 1953, Journal of experimental psychology.
[24] Peter Dayan,et al. A Neural Substrate of Prediction and Reward , 1997, Science.
[25] M. Denny,et al. Differential response learning on the basis of differential size of reward. , 1955, The Journal of Genetic Psychology.
[26] W. Pan,et al. Dopamine Cells Respond to Predicted Events during Classical Conditioning: Evidence for Eligibility Traces in the Reward-Learning Network , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[27] W. Pan,et al. Tripartite Mechanism of Extinction Suggested by Dopamine Neuron Activity and Temporal Difference Model , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[28] T. Kalenscher,et al. Single Units in the Pigeon Brain Integrate Reward Amount and Time-to-Reward in an Impulsive Choice Task , 2005, Current Biology.
[29] W. Schultz. Behavioral dopamine signals , 2007, Trends in Neurosciences.
[30] S. Grillner,et al. Forebrain dopamine depletion impairs motor behavior in lamprey , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.
[31] L. Crespi. Amount of reinforcement and level of performance. , 1944 .
[32] O. Güntürkün. Avian and mammalian “prefrontal cortices”: Limited degrees of freedom in the evolution of the neural mechanisms of goal-state maintenance , 2005, Brain Research Bulletin.
[33] T. Robbins,et al. Dopamine Release in the Dorsal Striatum during Cocaine-Seeking Behavior under the Control of a Drug-Associated Cue , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[34] P. J. Hutt. Rate of bar pressing as a function of quality and quantity of food reward. , 1954, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[35] W. Schultz,et al. Dopamine responses comply with basic assumptions of formal learning theory , 2001, Nature.
[36] K. Berridge. The debate over dopamine’s role in reward: the case for incentive salience , 2007, Psychopharmacology.
[37] G. Kimble,et al. Changes in response strength with changes in the amount of reinforcement. , 1956, Journal of experimental psychology.
[38] E. Vaadia,et al. Coincident but Distinct Messages of Midbrain Dopamine and Striatal Tonically Active Neurons , 2004, Neuron.
[39] G. Collier,et al. Changes in performance as a function of shifts in the magnitude of reinforcement. , 1959, Journal of experimental psychology.
[40] W. O. Jenkins,et al. Rate of responding and amount of reinforcement. , 1949, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[41] W. Schultz,et al. Adaptive Coding of Reward Value by Dopamine Neurons , 2005, Science.
[42] T. Robbins,et al. Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsion , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[43] Ziv M. Williams,et al. Selective enhancement of associative learning by microstimulation of the anterior caudate , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.