The Influence of Age and Work-Related Expertise on Fine Motor Control

Age-related decline of fine motor control commences even in middle adulthood. Less is known, however, whether age-related changes can be postponed through continuous practice. In this study we tested how age and professional expertise influence fine motor control in middle-aged adults. Forty-eight right-handed novices and experts (35 to 65 years) performed submaximal precision grip force modulation tasks with index or middle finger opposing the thumb, either with the right hand or the left hand. Novices revealed expected age-related differences in all performance measures (force initialization, mean applied force, variability), whereas experts outperformed novices in all outcome measures. Expertise seems to contribute to maintaining manual skills into older age, as indicated by the age and expertise interaction for the force initialization.

[1]  James D. Miles,et al.  The role of cognitive stimulation on the relations between age and cognitive functioning. , 2002, Psychology and aging.

[2]  Marc A. Maier,et al.  Precision in isometric precision grip force is reduced in middle-aged adults , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  Tara L. McIsaac,et al.  Finger Force Coordination Underlying Object Manipulation in the Elderly – A Mini-Review , 2010, Gerontology.

[4]  Claudia Voelcker-Rehage,et al.  Age-related Differences in Working Memory and Force Control under Dual-task Conditions , 2006, Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition.

[5]  R. Kane,et al.  Manual Dexterity as a Correlate of Dependency in the Elderly , 1989, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[6]  Quinn Kennedy,et al.  Age and expertise effects in aviation decision making and flight control in a flight simulator. , 2010, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[7]  Reinhold Kliegl,et al.  The Effects of Expertise and Age on Rhythm Production: Adaptations to Timing and Sequencing Constraints , 2002, Brain and Cognition.

[8]  Hubert R. Dinse,et al.  Age-Related Attenuation of Dominant Hand Superiority , 2006, PloS one.

[9]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  Joseph T. Gwin,et al.  Motor control and aging: Links to age-related brain structural, functional, and biochemical effects , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[11]  R. Krampe Aging, expertise and fine motor movement , 2002, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[12]  R. Enoka,et al.  Reduced control of motor output in a human hand muscle of elderly subjects during submaximal contractions. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  K. A. Ericsson,et al.  Toward a general theory of expertise : prospects and limits , 1991 .

[14]  K. J. Cole,et al.  Tactile impairments cannot explain the effect of age on a grasp and lift task , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  K. Newell,et al.  The generalization of perceptual-motor intra-individual variability in young and old adults. , 2006, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[16]  Eleftherios Kellis,et al.  Force variability during isometric wrist flexion in highly skilled and sedentary individuals , 2009, European Journal of Applied Physiology.

[17]  Jörg Schorer,et al.  Expertise and aging: maintaining skills through the lifespan , 2008, European Review of Aging and Physical Activity.

[18]  Leonardo Bonilha,et al.  Practice and perfect: length of training and structural brain changes in experienced typists , 2007, Neuroreport.

[19]  S. Morrison,et al.  Age-related differences in inter-digit coupling during finger pinching , 2006, European Journal of Applied Physiology.

[20]  Eric A. Roy,et al.  A Performance Measure of the Degree of Hand Preference , 2000, Brain and Cognition.

[21]  M. Goodale,et al.  Hand preference for precision grasping predicts language lateralization , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  G. Yue,et al.  Effects of Aging on Hand Function , 2001, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[23]  B. Godde,et al.  Fit between workers’ competencies and job demands as predictor for job performance over the work career , 2011 .

[24]  Jay L. Alberts,et al.  Age-related changes in grasping force modulation , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  H. Simon,et al.  Skill in Chess , 1988 .

[26]  Edward Ofori,et al.  Age-related differences in force variability and visual display , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[27]  B. Rockstroh,et al.  Increased Cortical Representation of the Fingers of the Left Hand in String Players , 1995, Science.

[28]  Sabine Sonnentag,et al.  Deliberate practice at work: A study with insurance agents , 2000 .

[29]  Kelly J. Cole,et al.  Age-related directional bias of fingertip force , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[30]  K. A. Ericsson,et al.  Maintaining excellence: deliberate practice and elite performance in young and older pianists. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[31]  Lara A. Boyd,et al.  Aging effects on the control of grip force magnitude: An fMRI study , 2011, Experimental Gerontology.

[32]  Caroline Moor,et al.  The Functional Quality of Life (fQOL)-Model A New Basis for Quality of Life-Enhancing Interventions in Old Age , 2012 .

[33]  T. Salthouse Effects of age and skill in typing. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[34]  R. C. Oldfield THE ASSESSMENT AND ANALYSIS OF HANDEDNESS , 1971 .

[35]  A. Schleicher,et al.  Motor cortex and hand motor skills: Structural compliance in the human brain , 1997, Human brain mapping.

[36]  Lutz Jäncke,et al.  Hand Skill Asymmetry in Professional Musicians , 1997, Brain and Cognition.

[37]  Marliese Kimmerle,et al.  The functional repertoire of the hand and its application to assessment. , 2003, The American journal of occupational therapy : official publication of the American Occupational Therapy Association.

[38]  Ben Godde,et al.  Touch perception throughout working life: effects of age and expertise , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.