Accumulating Evidence About What Prospective Memory Costs Actually Reveal

Event-based prospective memory (PM) tasks require participants to substitute an atypical PM response for an ongoing task response when presented with PM targets. Responses to ongoing tasks are often slower with the addition of PM demands (“PM costs”). Prominent PM theories attribute costs to capacity-sharing between the ongoing and PM tasks, which reduces the rate of processing of the ongoing task. We modeled PM costs using the Linear Ballistic Accumulator and the Diffusion Decision Model in a lexical-decision task with nonfocal PM targets defined by semantic categories. Previous decision modeling, which attributed costs to changes in caution rather than rate of processing (Heathcote et al., 2015; Horn & Bayen, 2015), could be criticized on the grounds that the PM tasks included did not sufficiently promote capacity-sharing. Our semantic PM task was potentially more dependent on lexical decision resources than previous tasks (Marsh, Hicks, & Cook, 2005), yet costs were again driven by changes in threshold and not by changes in processing speed (drift rate). Costs resulting from a single target focal PM task were also driven by threshold changes. The increased thresholds underlying nonfocal and focal costs were larger for word trials than nonword trials. As PM targets were always words, this suggests that threshold increases are used to extend the time available for retrieval on PM trials. Under nonfocal conditions, but not focal conditions, the nonword threshold also increased. Thus, it seems that only nonfocal instructions cause a global threshold increase because of greater perceived task complexity.

[1]  Shayne Loft,et al.  Enhanced recognition of words previously presented in a task with nonfocal prospective memory requirements , 2012, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[2]  George Sperling,et al.  The spatial distribution of visual attention , 2004, Vision Research.

[3]  Jason L. Hicks,et al.  Metacognitive awareness of event-based prospective memory , 2007, Consciousness and Cognition.

[4]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Normal aging and prospective memory. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[5]  F. Vitu,et al.  The Optimal Viewing Position effect in the lower visual field , 2013, Vision Research.

[6]  Shayne Loft,et al.  Wait a second: Brief delays in responding reduce focality effects in event-based prospective memory , 2013, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[7]  G. Logan,et al.  On the ability to inhibit thought and action: general and special theories of an act of control. , 2014, Psychological review.

[8]  N. Unsworth,et al.  Working memory capacity: Attention control, secondary memory, or both? A direct test of the dual-component model , 2010 .

[9]  Shayne Loft,et al.  Slow down and remember to remember! A delay theory of prospective memory costs. , 2015, Psychological review.

[10]  R. R. Hunt,et al.  The cost of event-based prospective memory: salient target events. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[11]  C. Bundesen A theory of visual attention. , 1990, Psychological review.

[12]  Jan Rummel,et al.  A diffusion model analysis of task interference effects in prospective memory , 2012, Memory & cognition.

[13]  Jason L Hicks,et al.  On the relationship between effort toward an ongoing task and cue detection in event-based prospective memory. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[14]  A. Welford THE ‘PSYCHOLOGICAL REFRACTORY PERIOD’ AND THE TIMING OF HIGH‐SPEED PERFORMANCE—A REVIEW AND A THEORY , 1952 .

[15]  E. C. Poulton,et al.  Influential companions: Effects of one strategy on another in the within-subjects designs of cognitive psychology. , 1982 .

[16]  H Pashler,et al.  Processing stages in overlapping tasks: evidence for a central bottleneck. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  U. Bayen,et al.  What can the diffusion model tell us about prospective memory? , 2011, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[18]  Joseph W. Houpt,et al.  Assessing the speed--accuracy trade-off effect on the capacity of information processing. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[19]  Jason L Hicks,et al.  The dynamics of intention retrieval and coordination of action in event-based prospective memory. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[20]  Mark A McDaniel,et al.  Focal/nonfocal cue effects in prospective memory: monitoring difficulty or different retrieval processes? , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[21]  R. Marsh,et al.  Implementation intentions about nonfocal event-based prospective memory tasks , 2010, Psychological research.

[22]  Peter M. Gollwitzer,et al.  The specificity of prospective memory costs , 2012, Memory.

[23]  Simon D Lilburn,et al.  Object selection costs in visual working memory: A diffusion model analysis of the focus of attention. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[24]  Birte U. Forstmann,et al.  On the efficiency of neurally-informed cognitive models to identify latent cognitive states , 2017 .

[25]  Rebekah E. Smith What Costs Do Reveal and Moving beyond the Cost Debate: Reply to Einstein and McDaniel (2010). , 2010 .

[26]  Shayne Loft,et al.  Applying Psychological Science to Examine Prospective Memory in Simulated Air Traffic Control , 2014 .

[27]  E. Wagenmakers,et al.  Diffusion versus linear ballistic accumulation: different models but the same conclusions about psychological processes? , 2010, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[28]  Jason L Hicks,et al.  Interference to ongoing activities covaries with the characteristics of an event-based intention. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[29]  J. O'Regan,et al.  Convenient fixation location within isolated words of different length and structure. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  E. Wagenmakers,et al.  AIC model selection using Akaike weights , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[31]  Andrew Heathcote,et al.  Converging measures of workload capacity , 2010, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[32]  Shayne Loft,et al.  Control of access to memory: The use of task interference as a behavioral probe , 2008 .

[33]  Lila Davachi,et al.  Are Representations in Working Memory Distinct From Representations in Long-Term Memory? , 2010, Psychological science.

[34]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Strategic and automatic processes in prospective memory retrieval: a multiprocess framework , 2000 .

[35]  U. Bayen,et al.  A multinomial model of event-based prospective memory. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[36]  J. Hicks,et al.  Encountering items previously paired with prospective memory target events can serve to reactivate intentions , 2012 .

[37]  Daniel Gopher,et al.  On the Economy of the Human Processing System: A Model of Multiple Capacity. , 1977 .

[38]  R. Marsh,et al.  Task interference in time-based, event-based, and dual intention prospective memory conditions☆ , 2005 .

[39]  Richard D. Morey,et al.  Confidence Intervals from Normalized Data: A correction to Cousineau (2005) , 2008 .

[40]  Michael Wilson,et al.  MRC psycholinguistic database: Machine-usable dictionary, version 2.00 , 1988 .

[41]  J T Townsend,et al.  Basic Response Time Tools for Studying General Processing Capacity in Attention, Perception, and Cognition , 2000, The Journal of general psychology.

[42]  Rebekah E. Smith The cost of remembering to remember in event-based prospective memory: investigating the capacity demands of delayed intention performance. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[43]  I. J. Myung,et al.  Applying Occam’s razor in modeling cognition: A Bayesian approach , 1997 .

[44]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  A comparison of sequential sampling models for two-choice reaction time. , 2004, Psychological review.

[45]  Bill Wilson,et al.  In a relationship , 2013 .

[46]  U. Bayen,et al.  Modeling criterion shifts and target checking in prospective memory monitoring. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[47]  Andrew Heathcote,et al.  Drawing conclusions from choice response time models: A tutorial using the linear ballistic accumulator , 2011 .

[48]  J. Townsend,et al.  An accuracy-response time capacity assessment function that measures performance against standard parallel predictions. , 2012, Psychological review.

[49]  Michael J Kane,et al.  Drifting from slow to "D'oh!": working memory capacity and mind wandering predict extreme reaction times and executive control errors. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[50]  F. Craik Levels of processing: Past, present... and future? , 2002, Memory.

[51]  Mark A. McDaniel,et al.  Dual pathways to prospective remembering , 2015, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[52]  Elizabeth A Maylor,et al.  Target context specification can reduce costs in nonfocal prospective memory. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[53]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Prospective Memory , 2005 .

[54]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Multiple processes in prospective memory retrieval: factors determining monitoring versus spontaneous retrieval. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[55]  R. Marsh,et al.  Event-based prospective memory and executive control of working memory. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[56]  U. Bayen,et al.  The effects of working memory resource availability on prospective memory: a formal modeling approach. , 2005, Experimental psychology.

[57]  Mark A McDaniel,et al.  Prospective memory and what costs do not reveal about retrieval processes: A commentary on Smith, Hunt, McVay, and McConnell (2007). , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[58]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  The simplest complete model of choice response time: Linear ballistic accumulation , 2008, Cognitive Psychology.

[59]  Daniel G Bobrow,et al.  On data-limited and resource-limited processes , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[60]  Paolo Ghisletta,et al.  Prospective memory and intraindividual variability in ongoing task response times in an adult lifespan sample: the role of cue focality , 2017, Memory.

[61]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Attention and Effort , 1973 .

[62]  Christopher D. Wickens,et al.  The Structure of Attentional Resources , 1980 .

[63]  I. J. Myung,et al.  Tutorial on maximum likelihood estimation , 2003 .