Subjective Experience Versus Content of Information in the Construction of Attitude Judgments

Experienced ease of thought generation was found to affect the impact of thought valence on attitude judgments. Participants generated either three or seven arguments that were either in favor or opposed to a specific issue. After generating three arguments, which was experienced as easy, participants' reported attitudes reflected the implications of their arguments, but this was not the case after generating seven arguments, which was experienced as difficult. In contrast, yoked participants who had read the arguments generated by the first group and consequently had lacked the subjective experience in thought generation were influenced more strongly by having been exposed to seven arguments rather than three. The results suggest that individuals use their subjective experience that accompany thought generation for the construction of their attitudes.

[1]  S. Chaiken,et al.  The generality of the automatic attitude activation effect. , 1992, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[2]  L. Jacoby,et al.  Becoming Famous Overnight: Limits on the Ability to Avoid Unconscious Influences of the Past , 1989 .

[3]  Yaacov Trope,et al.  Interpersonal comparison versus persuasive argumentation: A more direct test of alternative explanations for group-induced shifts in individual choice , 1973 .

[4]  G. Clore,et al.  Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. , 1983 .

[5]  J. Bargh,et al.  Social cognition and social perception. , 1987, Annual review of psychology.

[6]  J. Krosnick Attitude importance and attitude change , 1988 .

[7]  F. Strack,et al.  Ease of retrieval as information: Another look at the availability heuristic. , 1991 .

[8]  D. S. Lindsay,et al.  Remembering Mistaken for Knowing: Ease of Retrieval as a Basis for Confidence in Answers to General Knowledge Questions , 1993 .

[9]  G. Clore,et al.  Cognitive feelings and metacognitive judgments , 1994 .

[10]  Timothy C. Brock,et al.  Distraction Can Enhance or Reduce Yielding to Propaganda: Thought Disruption Versus Effort Justification , 1976 .

[11]  Carl Craig Morris,et al.  Retrieval processes underlying confidence in comprehension judgments. , 1990 .

[12]  A. Greenwald 6 – Cognitive Learning, Cognitive Response to Persuasion, and Attitude Change1 , 1968 .

[13]  Daniel Kahneman,et al.  Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability , 1973 .

[14]  Larry L. Jacoby,et al.  The construction of subjective experience: Memory attributions , 1990 .

[15]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Effects of message repetition and position on cognitive response, recall, and persuasion. , 1979 .

[16]  Shelley E. Taylor Judgment under uncertainty: The availability bias in social perception and interaction , 1982 .

[17]  A. Koriat How do we know that we know? The accessibility model of the feeling of knowing. , 1993 .

[18]  William J. McGuire,et al.  Some Contemporary Approaches , 1964 .

[19]  F. Strack,et al.  Thinking, Judging, and Communicating: A Process Account of Context Effects in Attitude Surveys , 1987 .

[20]  Ramon J. Rhine,et al.  Ego-involvement, discrepancy, source credibility, and attitude change. , 1970 .

[21]  A. Tesser Self-Generated Attitude Change , 1978 .

[22]  I. Janis,et al.  The influence of role playing on opinion change. , 1954, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[23]  Irving L. Janis,et al.  Comparison of the Effectiveness of Improvised Versus Non-Improvised Role-Playing in Producing Opinion Changes , 1956 .

[24]  N. Schwarz,et al.  The availability heuristic revisited : experienced ease of retrieval in mundane frequency estimates , 1995 .