The independency of the Bereitschaftspotential from previous stimulus-locked P3 in visuomotor response tasks.

The Bereitschaftspotential (BP) and the P3 are well-known ERPs usually observed during self-paced and externally triggered tasks. Recently, the BP was also detected in externally triggered tasks before stimulus onset. However, doubts have been raised about the authenticity of the BP in these tasks due to possible overlaps with the previous trial P3 (hereinafter s-1 P3). Here, we aim to test the authenticity of the BP in externally triggered tasks by comparing ERPs obtained during two visuomotor response tasks with different interstimulus intervals (ISIs) allowing (short-ISI; 1,000-2,000 ms) or not (long-ISI; 2,000-4,000 ms) P3-BP overlaps. In line with previous research, we hypothesize that BP and the s-1 P3 contribute independently to the scalp-detected activities during these tasks. ERPs were recorded from 14 healthy participants during the short-ISI and long-ISI visuomotor response tasks. Amplitudes and latencies of pre- (BP and pN) and poststimulus ERP components (P1, pN1, pP1, N1, pP2, dpP2, N2, P3) were compared between conditions. No effect of ISI was found on the amplitude of any pre- and poststimulus components. In contrast, longer ISI is associated with earlier onsets of the BP and pN components. In visuomotor response tasks, the BP is independent from the P3 elicited by the previous trial (s-1 P3), even using relatively short ISI. Since the different ISIs did not affect the amplitude of the BP and the P3 components, we conclude that also a short ISI can be adopted safely and conveniently to keep a reasonable duration of the overall experiment.

[1]  A. Mouraux,et al.  The Enhancement of the N1 Wave Elicited by Sensory Stimuli Presented at Very Short Inter-Stimulus Intervals Is a General Feature across Sensory Systems , 2008, PloS one.

[2]  P. Lang,et al.  Cortical slow-wave and cardiac rate responses in stimulus orientation and reaction time conditions. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.

[3]  Jorge Leite,et al.  Mind Wandering and Task-Focused Attention: ERP Correlates , 2018, Scientific Reports.

[4]  R. Iansek,et al.  Movement-related potentials in Parkinson's disease. Presence and predictability of temporal and spatial cues. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[5]  Ewald Moser,et al.  The preparation and readiness for voluntary movement: a high-field event-related fMRI study of the Bereitschafts-BOLD response , 2003, NeuroImage.

[6]  D. Spinelli,et al.  Awareness affects motor planning for goal-oriented actions , 2012, Biological Psychology.

[7]  F. Russo,et al.  Exercise-related cognitive effects on sensory-motor control in athletes and drummers compared to non-athletes and other musicians , 2017, Neuroscience.

[8]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Foreperiod and simple reaction time. , 1981 .

[9]  C. Gonsalvez,et al.  Target-to-target interval, intensity, and P300 from an auditory single-stimulus task. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[10]  H Shibasaki,et al.  Subdural potentials at orbitofrontal and mesial prefrontal areas accompanying anticipation and decision making in humans: a comparison with Bereitschaftspotential. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[11]  L. Deecke,et al.  The Preparation and Execution of Self-Initiated and Externally-Triggered Movement: A Study of Event-Related fMRI , 2002, NeuroImage.

[12]  Gaspare Galati,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetries in the transition from action preparation to execution , 2017, NeuroImage.

[13]  J. Verhoeven,et al.  Developmental Foreign Accent Syndrome: Report of a New Case , 2016, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[14]  Donatella Spinelli,et al.  Spatiotemporal brain mapping during preparation, perception, and action , 2016, NeuroImage.

[15]  M. Berchicci,et al.  Beyond the “Bereitschaftspotential”: Action preparation behind cognitive functions , 2017, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[16]  V. Bianco,et al.  The proactive self-control of actions: Time-course of underlying brain activities , 2017, NeuroImage.

[17]  W. Walter,et al.  Contingent Negative Variation : An Electric Sign of Sensori-Motor Association and Expectancy in the Human Brain , 1964, Nature.

[18]  F. Russo,et al.  Weak proactive cognitive/motor brain control accounts for poor children’s behavioral performance in speeded discrimination tasks , 2018, Biological Psychology.

[19]  J. Polich,et al.  P300, probability, and interstimulus interval. , 1990, Psychophysiology.

[20]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  D. Spinelli,et al.  Individual differences in response speed and accuracy are associated to specific brain activities of two interacting systems , 2014, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[22]  C. Marsden,et al.  Self-initiated versus externally triggered movements. I. An investigation using measurement of regional cerebral blood flow with PET and movement-related potentials in normal and Parkinson's disease subjects. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[23]  Myung Yung Jeong,et al.  Identification and Removal of Physiological Artifacts From Electroencephalogram Signals: A Review , 2018, IEEE Access.

[24]  E. Donchin,et al.  On the independence of the CNV and the P300 components of the human averaged evoked potential. , 1975, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[25]  Werner Sommer,et al.  Updating and validating a new framework for restoring and analyzing latency-variable ERP components from single trials with residue iteration decomposition (RIDE). , 2015, Psychophysiology.

[26]  R. Verleger,et al.  Time to Move Again: Does the Bereitschaftspotential Covary with Demands on Internal Timing? , 2016, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[27]  W. Sommer,et al.  Effects of inter-stimulus interval on skin conductance responses and event-related potentials in a Go/NoGo task , 2009, Biological Psychology.

[28]  T W Picton,et al.  Temporal and sequential probability in evoked potential studies. , 1981, Canadian journal of psychology.

[29]  Sven Hoffmann,et al.  The Correction of Eye Blink Artefacts in the EEG: A Comparison of Two Prominent Methods , 2008, PloS one.

[30]  R. Passingham,et al.  Self-initiated versus externally triggered movements. II. The effect of movement predictability on regional cerebral blood flow. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[31]  Jan R. Wessel,et al.  Prepotent motor activity and inhibitory control demands in different variants of the go/no-go paradigm. , 2018, Psychophysiology.

[32]  Donatella Spinelli,et al.  Awareness of perception and sensory–motor integration: ERPs from the anterior insula , 2018, Brain Structure and Function.

[33]  J. Polich,et al.  Cognitive and biological determinants of P300: an integrative review , 1995, Biological Psychology.

[34]  S. Kelly,et al.  A supramodal accumulation-to-bound signal that determines perceptual decisions in humans , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.

[35]  Marika Berchicci,et al.  Different proactive and reactive action control in fencers’ and boxers’ brain , 2017, Neuroscience.

[36]  M. Berchicci,et al.  New insights into old waves. Matching stimulus- and response-locked ERPs on the same time-window , 2016, Biological Psychology.

[37]  P. Brakefield,et al.  The Male Sex Pheromone of the Butterfly Bicyclus anynana: Towards an Evolutionary Analysis , 2008, PloS one.

[38]  J. Polich Probability and inter-stimulus interval effects on the P300 from auditory stimuli. , 1990, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[39]  Nick C Fox,et al.  Gene-Wide Analysis Detects Two New Susceptibility Genes for Alzheimer's Disease , 2014, PLoS ONE.

[40]  R. Verleger Event-related potentials and cognition: A critique of the context updating hypothesis and an alternative interpretation of P3 , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[41]  E Gordon,et al.  Is the target-to-target interval a critical determinant of P3 amplitude? , 1999, Psychophysiology.

[42]  J. Polich Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[43]  Donatella Spinelli,et al.  Prefrontal hyperactivity in older people during motor planning , 2012, NeuroImage.

[44]  H. Kornhuber,et al.  Hirnpotentialänderungen bei Willkürbewegungen und passiven Bewegungen des Menschen: Bereitschaftspotential und reafferente Potentiale , 1965, Pflüger's Archiv für die gesamte Physiologie des Menschen und der Tiere.

[45]  Donatella Spinelli,et al.  The premotor role of the prefrontal cortex in response consistency. , 2015, Neuropsychology.

[46]  D. Spinelli,et al.  Brain waves from an “isolated” cortex: contribution of the anterior insula to cognitive functions , 2017, Brain Structure and Function.

[47]  F. Aboitiz,et al.  Temporal Constraints of Behavioral Inhibition: Relevance of Inter-stimulus Interval in a Go-Nogo Task , 2014, PloS one.

[48]  Michael C. Anderson,et al.  The Prefrontal Cortex Achieves Inhibitory Control by Facilitating Subcortical Motor Pathway Connectivity , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  E. Vogel,et al.  Electrophysiological Evidence for a Postperceptual Locus of Suppression during the Attentional Blink Time-based Attention and the Attentional Blink , 1998 .

[50]  K. Böcker,et al.  Cortical Measures of Anticipation , 2004 .

[51]  Donatella Spinelli,et al.  Missing the Target: the Neural Processing Underlying the Omission Error , 2017, Brain Topography.

[52]  W. Sommer,et al.  Stimulus presentation rate dissociates sequential effects in event-related potentials and reaction times. , 1993, Psychophysiology.

[53]  M. Hallett,et al.  What is the Bereitschaftspotential? , 2006, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[54]  Rolf Verleger,et al.  Is P3 a strategic or a tactical component? Relationships of P3 sub-components to response times in oddball tasks with go, no-go and choice responses , 2016, NeuroImage.

[55]  L. Maffei,et al.  Environmental enrichment strengthens corticocortical interactions and reduces amyloid-β oligomers in aged mice , 2013, Front. Aging Neurosci..

[56]  John Polich,et al.  P300 Sequence Effects, Probability, and Interstimulus Interval , 1997, Physiology & Behavior.

[57]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  Removing electroencephalographic artifacts by blind source separation. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[58]  Donatella Spinelli,et al.  Benefits of Physical Exercise on Basic Visuo-Motor Functions Across Age , 2014, Front. Aging Neurosci..

[59]  G. Thickbroom,et al.  The role of the supplementary motor area in externally timed movement: the influence of predictability of movement timing , 2000, Brain Research.

[60]  C. Brunia,et al.  Distribution of slow brain potentials related to motor preparation and stimulus anticipation in a time estimation task. , 1988, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[61]  Donatella Spinelli,et al.  Why do we make mistakes? Neurocognitive processes during the preparation–perception–action cycle and error-detection , 2015, NeuroImage.

[62]  R. Iansek,et al.  Movement-related potentials in Parkinson's disease. Motor imagery and movement preparation. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[63]  E. Donchin Presidential address, 1980. Surprise!...Surprise? , 1981, Psychophysiology.

[64]  Michael Falkenstein,et al.  Diversity of the P3 in the task-switching paradigm , 2011, Brain Research.