Representation of Color Surfaces in V1: Edge Enhancement and Unfilled Holes

The neuronal mechanism underlying the representation of color surfaces in primary visual cortex (V1) is not well understood. We tested on color surfaces the previously proposed hypothesis that visual perception of uniform surfaces is mediated by an isomorphic, filled-in representation in V1. We used voltage-sensitive-dye imaging in fixating macaque monkeys to measure V1 population responses to spatially uniform chromatic (red, green, or blue) and achromatic (black or white) squares of different sizes (0.5°–8°) presented for 300 ms. Responses to both color and luminance squares early after stimulus onset were similarly edge-enhanced: for squares 1° and larger, regions corresponding to edges were activated much more than those corresponding to the center. At later times after stimulus onset, responses to achromatic squares' centers increased, partially “filling-in” the V1 representation of the center. The rising phase of the center response was slower for larger squares. Surprisingly, the responses to color squares behaved differently. For color squares of all sizes, responses remained edge-enhanced throughout the stimulus. There was no filling-in of the center. Our results imply that uniform filled-in representations of surfaces in V1 are not required for the perception of uniform surfaces and that chromatic and achromatic squares are represented differently in V1. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT We used voltage-sensitive dye imaging from V1 of behaving monkeys to test the hypothesis that visual perception of uniform surfaces is mediated by an isomorphic, filled-in representation. We found that the early population responses to chromatic and achromatic surfaces are edge enhanced, emphasizing the importance of edges in surface processing. Next, we show for color surfaces that responses remained edge-enhanced throughout the stimulus presentation whereas response to luminance surfaces showed a slow neuronal ‘filling-in’ of the center. Our results suggest that isomorphic representation is not a general code for uniform surfaces in V1.

[1]  A. Hurlbert,et al.  Color contrast: a contributory mechanism to color constancy. , 2004, Progress in brain research.

[2]  Alex R. Wade,et al.  No Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Evidence for Brightness and Color Filling-In In Early Human Visual Cortex , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  Elie Bienenstock,et al.  Precise Spatiotemporal Patterns among Visual Cortical Areas and Their Relation to Visual Stimulus Processing , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  D. Macleod,et al.  Color appearance depends on the variance of surround colors , 1997, Current Biology.

[5]  Frank Tong,et al.  Filling-in of visual phantoms in the human brain , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[6]  D. G. Albrecht,et al.  Spatial mapping of monkey VI cells with pure color and luminance stimuli , 1984, Vision Research.

[7]  J. Pokorny,et al.  Spectral sensitivity of the foveal cone photopigments between 400 and 500 nm , 1975, Vision Research.

[8]  M. Paradiso,et al.  V1 response timing and surface filling-in. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  H. Komatsu The neural mechanisms of perceptual filling-in , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[10]  P. Lennie,et al.  The Impact of Suppressive Surrounds on Chromatic Properties of Cortical Neurons , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  Yi Wang,et al.  Representation of surface luminance and contrast in primary visual cortex. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[12]  D. G. Albrecht Visual cortex neurons in monkey and cat: Effect of contrast on the spatial and temporal phase transfer functions , 1995, Visual Neuroscience.

[13]  S. Shevell,et al.  Color in complex scenes. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.

[14]  Hamutal Slovin,et al.  Spatiotemporal effects of microsaccades on population activity in the visual cortex of monkeys during fixation. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[15]  Behzad Mansouri,et al.  Orientation variance discrimination in amblyopic and normal vision , 2004 .

[16]  Takeo Watanabe,et al.  The primary visual cortex fills in color , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[17]  Peter De Weerd,et al.  Responses of cells in monkey visual cortex during perceptual filling-in of an artificial scotoma , 1995, Nature.

[18]  Hamutal Slovin,et al.  A Contrast and Surface Code Explains Complex Responses to Black and White Stimuli in V1 , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  H. Komatsu,et al.  Neural representation of the luminance and brightness of a uniform surface in the macaque primary visual cortex. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  G. Kanizsa,et al.  Organization in Vision: Essays on Gestalt Perception , 1979 .

[21]  J. Victor,et al.  Population encoding of spatial frequency, orientation, and color in macaque V1. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  A. Leventhal,et al.  Concomitant sensitivity to orientation, direction, and color of cells in layers 2, 3, and 4 of monkey striate cortex , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[23]  F. Chavane,et al.  Imaging cortical correlates of illusion in early visual cortex , 2004, Nature.

[24]  R. Frostig,et al.  Cortical point-spread function and long-range lateral interactions revealed by real-time optical imaging of macaque monkey primary visual cortex , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[25]  B. Pinna,et al.  Surface color from boundaries: a new ‘watercolor’ illusion , 2001, Vision Research.

[26]  C. Hung,et al.  Building surfaces from borders in Areas 17 and 18 of the cat , 2001, Vision Research.

[27]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  Representation of Color Stimuli in Awake Macaque Primary Visual Cortex , 2003, Neuron.

[28]  Inbal Ayzenshtat,et al.  Population Response to Natural Images in the Primary Visual Cortex Encodes Local Stimulus Attributes and Perceptual Processing , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  C Wehrhahn,et al.  The Craik—O'Brien—Cornsweet Illusion in Colour: Quantitative Characterisation and Comparison with Luminance , 1997, Perception.

[30]  A. Grinvald,et al.  Long-term voltage-sensitive dye imaging reveals cortical dynamics in behaving monkeys. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  Ken Nakayama,et al.  Brightness perception and filling-in , 1991, Vision Research.

[32]  Bevil R. Conway,et al.  Spatial Structure of Cone Inputs to Color Cells in Alert Macaque Primary Visual Cortex (V-1) , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  V. Bringuier,et al.  Horizontal propagation of visual activity in the synaptic integration field of area 17 neurons. , 1999, Science.

[34]  Hong Zhou,et al.  The coding of uniform colour figures in monkey visual cortex , 2003, The Journal of physiology.

[35]  Hamutal Slovin,et al.  Population response to contextual influences in the primary visual cortex. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[36]  F. Chavane,et al.  Dynamics of Local Input Normalization Result from Balanced Short- and Long-Range Intracortical Interactions in Area V1 , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Microsaccades are triggered by low retinal image slip. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[38]  Victor A. F. Lamme,et al.  Separate processing dynamics for texture elements, boundaries and surfaces in primary visual cortex of the macaque monkey. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[39]  C. Hung,et al.  Cortical processing of a brightness illusion. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[40]  R. Shapley,et al.  The Orientation Selectivity of Color-Responsive Neurons in Macaque V1 , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  Peter B. Delahunt,et al.  Color constancy under changes in reflected illumination. , 2004, Journal of vision.

[42]  R. Shapley,et al.  The spatial transformation of color in the primary visual cortex of the macaque monkey , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[43]  A. Stockman,et al.  The spectral sensitivities of the middle- and long-wavelength-sensitive cones derived from measurements in observers of known genotype , 2000, Vision Research.

[44]  H. Komatsu,et al.  Neural Responses in the Retinotopic Representation of the Blind Spot in the Macaque V1 to Stimuli for Perceptual Filling-In , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  Gregory D Horwitz,et al.  V1 mechanisms underlying chromatic contrast detection. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  Michael A. Webster,et al.  Temporal properties of brightness and color induction , 1986, Vision Research.

[47]  K. Mullen The contrast sensitivity of human colour vision to red‐green and blue‐yellow chromatic gratings. , 1985, The Journal of physiology.

[48]  Peter De Weerd,et al.  Mechanisms of Surface Completion: Perceptual Filling-In of Texture , 2003 .

[49]  E. Switkes,et al.  Visual evoked potentials in three-dimensional color space: Correlates of spatio-chromatic processing , 1994, Vision Research.

[50]  Michael Breakspear,et al.  Modeling Magnification and Anisotropy in the Primate Foveal Confluence , 2010, PLoS Comput. Biol..