Interspecific Communication: Gaining Information from Heterospecific Alarm Calls
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Robert D Magrath,et al. A micro-geography of fear: learning to eavesdrop on alarm calls of neighbouring heterospecifics , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[2] Chaminda P. Ratnayake,et al. The frequency of vocal mimicry associated with danger varies due to proximity to nest and nesting stage in a passerine bird , 2014 .
[3] James S Adelman,et al. Heterospecific alarm call recognition in a non-vocal reptile , 2007, Biology Letters.
[4] S. Ghirlanda,et al. A century of generalization , 2003, Animal Behaviour.
[5] M. O’Riain,et al. Sharks shape the geometry of a selfish seal herd: experimental evidence from seal decoys , 2010, Biology Letters.
[6] U. Ernst,et al. The adaptive significance of avian mobbing: II. Cultural transmission of enemy recognition in blackbirds: Effectiveness and some constraints. , 2010 .
[8] Robert D. Magrath,et al. Functionally referential alarm calls in noisy miners communicate about predator behaviour , 2017, Animal Behaviour.
[9] E. Morton. Animal Vocal Communication: Assessment and Management Roles , 2017 .
[10] Yvonne Y. Chi,et al. Scared and less noisy: glucocorticoids are associated with alarm call entropy , 2012, Biology Letters.
[11] W. Shriner. Antipredator Responses to a Previously Neutral Sound by Free-living Adult Golden-mantled Ground Squirrels, Spermophilus lateralis (Sciuridae) , 1999 .
[12] Kartik Shanker,et al. Why do birds participate in mixed-species foraging flocks? A large-scale synthesis , 2009, Animal Behaviour.
[13] B. Pitcher,et al. Alarming features: birds use specific acoustic properties to identify heterospecific alarm calls , 2013, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[14] Eben Goodale,et al. Context-dependent vocal mimicry in a passerine bird , 2006, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[15] K. Zuberbühler,et al. Diana monkey long-distance calls: messages for conspecifics and predators , 1997, Animal Behaviour.
[16] E. Curio,et al. The hearing of an avian predator and its avian prey , 1986, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[17] Robert D. Magrath,et al. Calling in the Face of Danger: Predation Risk and Acoustic Communication by Parent Birds and Their Offspring , 2010 .
[18] S. Quader,et al. The nuclear question: rethinking species importance in multi-species animal groups. , 2010, The Journal of animal ecology.
[19] Laura A. Kelley,et al. The mimetic repertoire of the spotted bowerbird Ptilonorhynchus maculatus , 2011, Naturwissenschaften.
[20] M. Manser,et al. The information that receivers extract from alarm calls in suricates , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[21] J. M. Smith,et al. The Evolution of Alarm Calls , 1965, The American Naturalist.
[22] K. M. Kostan. The evolution of mutualistic interspecific communication: assessment and management across species. , 2002, Journal of Comparative Psychology.
[23] Janne-Tuomas Seppänen,et al. Interspecific Social Learning: Novel Preference Can Be Acquired from a Competing Species , 2007, Current Biology.
[24] K. Zuberbühler,et al. The Syntax and Meaning of Wild Gibbon Songs , 2006, PloS one.
[25] Andrea M.-K. Bierema,et al. On the Meaning of Alarm Calls: A Review of Functional Reference in Avian Alarm Calling , 2013 .
[26] Eben Goodale,et al. ALARM CALLING IN SRI LANKAN MIXED-SPECIES BIRD FLOCKS , 2005 .
[27] U. Ramakrishnan,et al. Recognition of heterospecific alarm vocalizations by bonnet macaques (Macaca radiata). , 2000, Journal of comparative psychology.
[28] Andrew M. Hindmarsh,et al. Vocal Mimicry in Starlings , 1984 .
[29] L. Ratcliffe,et al. Migrant and resident birds adjust antipredator behavior in response to social information accuracy , 2010 .
[30] M. Child,et al. Interspecific audience effects on the alarm-calling behaviour of a kleptoparasitic bird , 2007, Biology Letters.
[31] Jessica R. McLachlan,et al. Crying wolf to a predator: deceptive vocal mimicry by a bird protecting young , 2015, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[32] Trevor Murray,et al. Does signal deterioration compromise eavesdropping on other species' alarm calls? , 2015, Animal Behaviour.
[33] Toshitaka N. Suzuki. Parental alarm calls warn nestlings about different predatory threats , 2011, Current Biology.
[34] D. Blumstein,et al. A Test of the Nonlinearity Hypothesis in Great-tailed Grackles (Quiscalus mexicanus) , 2013 .
[35] R. D. Magrath,et al. Learning to listen? Nestling response to heterospecific alarm calls , 2012, Animal Behaviour.
[36] Miyoko Chu. HETEROSPECIFIC RESPONSES TO SCREAM CALLS AND VOCAL MIMICRY BY PHAINOPEPLAS (PHAINOPEPLA NITENS) IN DISTRESS , 2001 .
[37] Eben Goodale,et al. Response to conspecific and heterospecific alarm calls in mixed-species bird flocks of a Sri Lankan rainforest , 2008 .
[38] Benjamin J. Pitcher,et al. An avian eavesdropping network: alarm signal reliability and heterospecific response , 2009 .
[39] R. Haven Wiley,et al. 5 – Adaptations for Acoustic Communication in Birds: Sound Transmission and Signal Detection , 1982 .
[40] M. Ryan,et al. What do animal signals mean? , 2009, Animal Behaviour.
[41] Eben Goodale,et al. Vocal Mimicry of Alarm-Associated Sounds by a Drongo Elicits Flee and Mobbing Responses from Other Species that Participate in Mixed-Species Bird Flocks , 2014 .
[42] C. Munn. Birds that ‘cry wolf’ , 1986, Nature.
[43] David E. Gammon,et al. Northern Mockingbirds produce syntactical patterns of vocal mimicry that reflect taxonomy of imitated species , 2011 .
[44] Chaminda P. Ratnayake,et al. Two sympatric species of passerine birds imitate the same raptor calls in alarm contexts , 2009, Naturwissenschaften.
[45] A. Radford,et al. Eavesdropping on heterospecific alarm calls: from mechanisms to consequences , 2015, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.
[46] M. Enquist,et al. Darwin’s principle of antithesis revisited: a role for perceptual biases in the evolution of intraspecific signals , 1995, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[47] C. Vernon. VOCAL IMITATION BY SOUTHERN AFRICAN BIRDS , 1973 .
[48] R. D. Magrath,et al. Eavesdropping on the neighbours: fledglings learn to respond to heterospecific alarm calls , 2013, Animal Behaviour.
[49] Kathryn E. Sieving,et al. Antipredator calls of tufted titmice and interspecific transfer of encoded threat information , 2012 .
[50] M. Manser,et al. The information banded mongooses extract from heterospecific alarms , 2008, Animal Behaviour.
[51] K. Zuberbühler. Chapter 8 Survivor Signals , 2009 .
[52] N. Mondy,et al. The role of associative learning process on the response of fledgling great tits (Parus major) to mobbing calls , 2019, Animal Cognition.
[53] Cooperatively breeding Arabian babblers call differently when mobbing in different predator-induced situations , 1999 .
[54] A. Radford,et al. SINGING FOR YOUR SUPPER: SENTINEL CALLING BY KLEPTOPARASITES CAN MITIGATE THE COST TO VICTIMS , 2011, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.
[55] Robert D Magrath,et al. Recognition of other species' aerial alarm calls: speaking the same language or learning another? , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[56] P. Kappeler,et al. Anti-predator behavior of group-living Malagasy primates: mixed evidence for a referential alarm call system , 2002, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[57] J. Riegert,et al. Vocal mimicry in the song of Icterine warblers (Hippolais icterina): possible functions and sources of variability , 2018 .
[58] Ari E. Martínez,et al. Foraging guild influences dependence on heterospecific alarm calls in Amazonian bird flocks , 2012 .
[59] Kurt Hammerschmidt,et al. Functionally Referential Alarm Calls in Tamarins (Saguinus fuscicollis and Saguinus mystax) – Evidence from Playback Experiments , 2006 .
[60] P. Marler,et al. Monkey responses to three different alarm calls: evidence of predator classification and semantic communication. , 1980, Science.
[61] Hanspeter Herzel,et al. Calls out of chaos: the adaptive significance of nonlinear phenomena in mammalian vocal production , 2002, Animal Behaviour.
[62] R. Zann,et al. Mimetic song in superb lyrebirds: species mimicked and mimetic accuracy in different populations and age classes , 2008, Animal Behaviour.
[63] A. Møller. False alarm calls as a means of resource usurpation in the great tit Parus major , 2010 .
[64] K. Sanderson,et al. Alarm Vocalisations in Australian Birds: Convergent Characteristics and Phylogenetic Differences , 1994 .
[65] K. Zuberbühler,et al. Interspecies semantic communication in two forest primates , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[66] Graeme D Ruxton,et al. Interspecific information transfer influences animal community structure. , 2010, Trends in ecology & evolution.
[67] S. Nowicki,et al. The Evolution of Animal Communication: Reliability and Deception in Signaling Systems: Reliability and Deception in Signaling Systems , 2005 .
[68] E. Morton. Vocal mimicry in the Thick-billed Euphonia , 1976 .
[69] Torben Dabelsteen,et al. Animal Communication Networks: Public, private or anonymous? Facilitating and countering eavesdropping , 2005 .
[70] Laura A. Kelley,et al. Vocal mimicry in songbirds , 2008, Animal Behaviour.
[71] D. Parejo,et al. Alarm calls modulate the spatial structure of a breeding owl community , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[72] Marc Naguib,et al. Estimating the distance to a source of sound: mechanisms and adaptations for long-range communication , 2001, Animal Behaviour.
[73] T. Aubin. Why do distress calls evoke interspecific responses? An experimental study applied to some species of birds , 1991, Behavioural Processes.
[74] D. Blumstein,et al. What is the sound of fear? Behavioral responses of white-crowned sparrows Zonotrichia leucophrys to synthesized nonlinear acoustic phenomena , 2014 .
[75] Andrew N. Radford,et al. The development of alarm call behaviour in mammals and birds , 2009, Animal Behaviour.
[76] D. Blumstein,et al. Mechanisms of heterospecific recognition in avian mobbing calls , 2003 .
[77] Jessica R McLachlan,et al. Speedy revelations: how alarm calls can convey rapid, reliable information about urgent danger , 2020, Proceedings of the Royal Society B.
[78] Toshitaka N. Suzuki. Semantic communication in birds: evidence from field research over the past two decades , 2016, Ecological Research.
[79] Robert D. Magrath,et al. Wild Birds Learn to Eavesdrop on Heterospecific Alarm Calls , 2015, Current Biology.
[80] Simon W. Townsend,et al. Functionally Referential Communication in Mammals: The Past, Present and the Future , 2013 .
[81] Peter Marler,et al. Specific Distinctiveness in the Communication Signals of Birds , 1957 .
[82] Andrew Meade,et al. Detecting Regular Sound Changes in Linguistics as Events of Concerted Evolution , 2015, Current Biology.
[83] W. Hill,et al. Auditory Perception of Hawks and Owls for Passerine Alarm Calls , 2001 .
[84] D. McCullough,et al. Evolution of Alarm Signals in Ungulates with Special Reference to White-Tailed Deer , 1977, The American Naturalist.
[85] M. Manser,et al. The function of nonlinear phenomena in meerkat alarm calls , 2011, Biology Letters.
[86] Erick Greene,et al. Allometry of Alarm Calls: Black-Capped Chickadees Encode Information About Predator Size , 2005, Science.
[87] Marc D. Hauser,et al. How infant vervet monkeys learn to recognize starling alarm calls: The role of experience. , 1988 .
[88] Alexis C. Billings,et al. Steller’s jays assess and communicate about predator risk using detection cues and identity , 2017 .
[89] P. Sherman. Alarm calls of Belding's ground squirrels to aerial predators: nepotism or self-preservation? , 1985, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[90] J. Bradbury,et al. Principles of animal communication, 2nd ed. , 2011 .
[91] Eberhard Curio,et al. The adaptive significance of avian mobbing. III. Cultural transmission of enemy recognition in blackbirds: Cross-species tutoring and properties of learning , 1980, Animal Behaviour.
[92] N. Langmore,et al. Social learning of a brood parasite by its host , 2013, Biology Letters.
[93] T. Oatley. THE FUNCTIONS OF VOCAL IMITATION BY AFRICAN COSSYPHAS , 1969 .
[94] A. Flasskamp. The Adaptive Significance of Avian Mobbing V. An Experimental Test of the ‘Move On’ Hypothesis , 2010 .
[95] David Wheatcroft,et al. Repetition rate of calls used in multiple contexts communicates presence of predators to nestlings and adult birds , 2015, Animal Behaviour.
[96] David Wheatcroft,et al. Learning and signal copying facilitate communication among bird species , 2013, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[97] D. Blumstein,et al. Sounds Scary? Lack of Habituation following the Presentation of Novel Sounds , 2011, PloS one.
[98] B. Igic,et al. A songbird mimics different heterospecific alarm calls in response to different types of threat , 2014 .
[99] Anastasia H. Dalziell,et al. Fooling the experts: accurate vocal mimicry in the song of the superb lyrebird, Menura novaehollandiae , 2012, Animal Behaviour.
[100] T. Krama,et al. Mobbing behaviour: reciprocity‐based co‐operation in breeding Pied Flycatchers Ficedula hypoleuca , 2006 .
[101] M. Manser,et al. The function of non-linear phenomena in meerkat alarm calls , 2011 .
[102] Robert D. Magrath,et al. Eavesdropping on other species: mutual interspecific understanding of urgency information in avian alarm calls , 2010, Animal Behaviour.
[103] Michael Griesser,et al. Referential Calls Signal Predator Behavior in a Group-Living Bird Species , 2008, Current Biology.
[104] A. S. Griffin,et al. Social learning about predators: a review and prospectus , 2004, Learning & behavior.
[105] David Wheatcroft,et al. Rates of signal evolution are associated with the nature of interspecific communication , 2015 .
[106] T. Lengagne,et al. Mobbing calls: a signal transcending species boundaries , 2017, Animal Behaviour.
[107] T. Peake,et al. Animal Communication Networks: Eavesdropping in communication networks , 2005 .
[108] A. Ridley,et al. Deception by Flexible Alarm Mimicry in an African Bird , 2014, Science.
[109] P. Marler,et al. Characteristics of Some Animal Calls , 1955, Nature.
[110] R. Oda,et al. Interspecific Responses of Ringtailed Lemurs to Playback of Antipredator Alarm Calls Given by Verreaux's Sifakas , 2010 .
[111] P. Marler,et al. Vervet monkey alarm calls: Semantic communication in a free-ranging primate , 1980, Animal Behaviour.
[112] I. J. Rooke,et al. Alarm calls of Honeyeaters with reference to locating sources of sound , 1977 .
[113] Jessica R. McLachlan,et al. Personal information about danger trumps social information from avian alarm calls , 2019, Proceedings of the Royal Society B.
[114] W. Cresswell. The function of alarm calls in redshanks, Tringa totanus , 1994, Animal Behaviour.
[115] K. Sanderson,et al. A comparative analysis of distress call structure in Australian passerine and non-passerine species : influence of size and phylogeny , 1998 .
[116] Toshitaka N Suzuki,et al. Experimental evidence for compositional syntax in bird calls , 2016, Nature Communications.
[117] D. Blumstein,et al. The Sound of Arousal: The Addition of Novel Non-linearities Increases Responsiveness in Marmot Alarm Calls , 2009 .
[118] Sasha R. X. Dall,et al. The ecology of information: an overview on the ecological significance of making informed decisions , 2010 .
[119] Klaus Zuberbühler,et al. Causal knowledge of predators' behaviour in wild Diana monkeys , 2000, Animal Behaviour.
[120] T. Krama,et al. Interspecific reciprocity explains mobbing behaviour of the breeding chaffinches, Fringilla coelebs , 2002, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[121] G. Klump,et al. Acoustic Behaviour of Birds and Mammals in the Predator Context; I. Factors Affecting the Structure of Alarm Signals. II. The Functional Significance and Evolution of Alarm Signals , 2010 .
[122] D. Franks,et al. Multiple adaptive and non-adaptive processes determine responsiveness to heterospecific alarm calls in African savannah herbivores , 2018, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[123] T. Freeberg,et al. Distress calls in tufted titmice (Baeolophus bicolor): are conspecifics or predators the target? , 2012 .
[124] W. Searcy,et al. Eavesdropping and cue denial in avian acoustic signals , 2017, Animal Behaviour.
[125] A. J. Marshall. The function of vocal mimicry in birds. , 1950 .
[126] J. S. Greenlaw,et al. Call mimicry by Eastern Towhees and its significance in relation to auditory learning , 1998 .
[127] Chaminda P. Ratnayake,et al. Birds Learn Socially to Recognize Heterospecific Alarm Calls by Acoustic Association , 2018, Current Biology.
[128] T. Caro,et al. Antipredator Defenses in Birds and Mammals , 2006 .
[129] R. Haven Wiley,et al. Noise Matters: The Evolution of Communication , 2015 .
[130] D. Blumstein,et al. Individual, age and sex-specific information is contained in yellow-bellied marmot alarm calls , 2005, Animal Behaviour.
[131] S. Gill,et al. Nest Defence By Yellow Warblers: Recognition of a Brood Parasite and an Avian Nest Predator , 1996 .
[132] J. Gardner,et al. Sound familiar? Acoustic similarity provokes responses to unfamiliar heterospecific alarm calls , 2011 .
[133] S. Zollinger,et al. Motor mechanisms of a vocal mimic: implications for birdsong production , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[134] Robert M. Seyfarth,et al. Suricate alarm calls signal predator class and urgency , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[135] M. Manser,et al. Nonlinearities in Meerkat Alarm Calls Prevent Receivers from Habituating , 2014 .
[136] T. Flower. Fork-tailed drongos use deceptive mimicked alarm calls to steal food , 2011, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[137] F. Knowlton,et al. Response of coyotes to avian distress calls : testing the startle-predator and predator-attraction hypotheses , 1999 .
[138] J. Mappes,et al. Imperfect Batesian mimicry—the effects of the frequency and the distastefulness of the model , 1997, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[139] Lucy F. Farrow,et al. Alarm calls of a cooperative bird are referential and elicit context-specific antipredator behavior , 2017 .
[140] M. Ficken,et al. A COMPARATIVE ANALYSIS OF PASSERINE MOBBING CALLS , 1996 .
[141] B. Igic,et al. Avian vocal mimicry: a unified conceptual framework , 2015, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.
[142] Mikko Mönkkönen,et al. Social information use is a process across time, space, and ecology, reaching heterospecifics. , 2007, Ecology.
[143] T. Aubin. The Role of Frequency Modulation in the Process of Distress Calls Recognition By the Starling (Sturnus Vulgaris) , 1989 .
[144] Richard A. Pettifor,et al. The effects of avian mobbing on a potential predator, the European kestrel, Falco tinnunculus , 1990, Animal Behaviour.
[145] The significance of antecedent auditory experiences on later reactions to the "seeet" alarm-call in great tit nestlings Parus major. , 2010, Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie.
[146] A. Ridley,et al. The ecological benefits of interceptive eavesdropping , 2014 .
[147] Olof O. Rydéan. Selective resistance to approach: a precursor to fear responses to an alarm call in great tit nestlings Parus major. , 1982 .
[148] T. Flower,et al. Kleptoparasitism by attacks versus false alarm calls in fork-tailed drongos , 2012, Animal Behaviour.
[149] J. Welbergen,et al. Elaborate Mimetic Vocal Displays by Female Superb Lyrebirds , 2016, Front. Ecol. Evol..
[150] C. Randler,et al. Heterospecifics do not respond to subtle differences in chaffinch mobbing calls: message is encoded in number of elements , 2011, Animal Behaviour.
[151] A. Mori,et al. A Day Gecko Darkens its Body Color in Response to Avian Alarm Calls , 2013 .
[152] A. Mori,et al. Vigilance against predators induced by eavesdropping on heterospecific alarm calls in a non-vocal lizard Oplurus cuvieri cuvieri (Reptilia: Iguania) , 2010, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[153] Dominique A. Potvin,et al. Birds orient their heads appropriately in response to functionally referential alarm calls of heterospecifics , 2018, Animal Behaviour.
[154] P. Sherman. Nepotism and the evolution of alarm calls. , 1977, Science.
[155] Robert D. Magrath,et al. Communicating about danger: urgency alarm calling in a bird , 2005, Animal Behaviour.
[156] Erick Greene,et al. Nuthatches eavesdrop on variations in heterospecific chickadee mobbing alarm calls , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[157] K. Sullivan. Selective alarm calling by downy woodpeckers in mixed-species flocks , 1985 .
[158] B. Igic,et al. Fidelity of vocal mimicry: identification and accuracy of mimicry of heterospecific alarm calls by the brown thornbill , 2013, Animal Behaviour.
[159] C. Darwin,et al. The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals , 1872 .
[160] P. Slater,et al. Hornbills can distinguish between primate alarm calls , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[161] M. Manser,et al. The acoustic structure of suricates' alarm calls varies with predator type and the level of response urgency , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[162] D. Neudorf,et al. Distress Calls of Birds in a Neotropical Cloud Forest 1 , 2002 .
[163] E. Morton. On the Occurrence and Significance of Motivation-Structural Rules in Some Bird and Mammal Sounds , 1977, The American Naturalist.
[164] Carel ten Cate,et al. Biases in signal evolution: learning makes a difference. , 2007, Trends in ecology & evolution.