Post-stop-signal adjustments: inhibition improves subsequent inhibition.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Bernhard Hommel,et al. Interactions between stimulus-stimulus congruence and stimulus-response compatibility , 1997 .
[2] E. Tulving,et al. Organization of memory. , 1973 .
[3] Martina Rieger,et al. Inhibitory after‐effects in the stop signal paradigm , 1999 .
[4] Rajesh P. N. Rao,et al. A rational decision making framework for inhibitory control , 2010, NIPS.
[5] G. Logan,et al. Impulsivity and Inhibitory Control , 1997 .
[6] Marcel Brass,et al. When doing nothing is an option: The neural correlates of deciding whether to act or not , 2009, NeuroImage.
[7] G. Logan. On the ability to inhibit thought and action , 1984 .
[8] J. Schall,et al. Executive control of countermanding saccades by the supplementary eye field , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.
[9] T. Wager,et al. Motivational influences on response inhibition measures. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[10] Darryl W. Schneider,et al. Automatic and Controlled Response Inhibition: Associative Learning in the Go/no-go and Stop-signal Paradigms the Go/no-go Paradigm and the Stop-signal Paradigm , 2022 .
[11] Elizabeth B. Liddle,et al. Looking before you leap: A theory of motivated control of action , 2009, Cognition.
[12] William M. Hartmann,et al. Psychoacoustics: Facts and Models , 2001 .
[13] W. Neill,et al. Persistence of negative priming: II. Evidence for episodic trace retrieval. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[14] L. Pessoa,et al. Interactions between cognition and motivation during response inhibition , 2010, Neuropsychologia.
[15] W. Neill,et al. Persistence of negative priming: Steady state or decay? , 1992 .
[16] Gordon D Logan,et al. Evidence for an Error Monitoring Deficit in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder , 2004, Journal of abnormal child psychology.
[17] M. Botvinick,et al. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.
[18] Patrick G. Bissett,et al. Post-stop-signal slowing: strategies dominate reflexes and implicit learning. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[19] W. Neill,et al. Persistence of negative priming: II. Evidence for episodic trace retrieval , 1992 .
[20] I. Franks,et al. Predictability influences stopping and response control. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[21] T. Palmeri. Exemplar similarity and the development of automaticity. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[22] Donald Laming,et al. Information theory of choice-reaction times , 1968 .
[23] R. Nosofsky. American Psychological Association, Inc. Choice, Similarity, and the Context Theory of Classification , 2022 .
[24] T. K. Srull,et al. Content and Process Specificity in the Effects of Prior Experiences , 1990 .
[25] Frederick Verbruggen,et al. Long-term aftereffects of response inhibition: memory retrieval, task goals, and cognitive control. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[26] H. Levitt. Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. , 1971, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[27] G. Logan. Toward an instance theory of automatization. , 1988 .
[28] Joshua W. Brown,et al. Performance Monitoring by the Anterior Cingulate Cortex During Saccade Countermanding , 2003, Science.
[29] W. Notebaert,et al. Effects of stimulus-stimulus compatibility and stimulus-response compatibility on response inhibition. , 2005, Acta psychologica.
[30] Frederick Verbruggen,et al. Short-term aftereffects of response inhibition: repetition priming or between-trial control adjustments? , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[31] P. Rabbitt. Errors and error correction in choice-response tasks. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.
[32] M. Posner,et al. Attention and cognitive control. , 1975 .
[33] Gordon D Logan,et al. Reduced response readiness delays stop signal inhibition. , 2002, Acta psychologica.
[34] R. Biddulph,et al. Differential Pitch Sensitivity of the Ear , 1931 .
[35] Russell A. Poldrack,et al. Inhibition-related Activation in the Right Inferior Frontal Gyrus in the Absence of Inhibitory Cues , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[36] E. Tulving,et al. Episodic and semantic memory , 1972 .
[37] Leanne Boucher,et al. Nonindependent and nonstationary response times in stopping and stepping saccade tasks , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[38] G. Logan. Repetition priming and automaticity: Common underlying mechanisms? , 1990, Cognitive Psychology.
[39] Patrick G. Bissett,et al. Dissociating interference-control processes between memory and response. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[40] Patrick G. Bissett,et al. Balancing Cognitive Demands: Control Adjustments in the Stop-signal Paradigm Stop-signal Paradigm Post-stop-signal Slowing , 2011 .
[41] Alan S. Brown,et al. Information Processing and Cognition: The Loyola Symposium , 1976 .
[42] B. Hommel,et al. A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task , 2004, Psychological research.
[43] D. Meyer,et al. A Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1993 .
[44] T. Zandt. Analysis of Response Time Distributions , 2002 .
[45] Robert S. Wyer,et al. On the Indistinguishability of Exemplar Memory and Abstraction in Category Representation , 2014 .
[46] C. Eriksen,et al. Use of a delayed signal to stop a visual reaction-time response. , 1966 .
[47] L. Barsalou. On the indistinguishability of exemplar memory and abstraction in category representation , 1990 .
[48] E. Awh,et al. Conflict adaptation effects in the absence of executive control , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[49] Wolfgang Prinz,et al. Busy doing nothing: Evidence for nonaction-effect binding , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[50] Hugo Fastl,et al. Psychoacoustics: Facts and Models , 1990 .
[51] W. Neill,et al. Persistence of negative priming: Steady state or decay? , 1992 .
[52] Erik M Altmann,et al. Testing probability matching and episodic retrieval accounts of response repetition effects in task switching. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[53] G. Logan. Executive control of thought and action , 1985 .
[54] KongFatt Wong-Lin,et al. Optimal performance in a countermanding saccade task , 2010, Brain Research.
[55] G. Logan,et al. On the ability to inhibit thought and action: general and special theories of an act of control. , 2014, Psychological review.
[56] Bryan Rodgers,et al. What does a Man do after he Makes an Error? An Analysis of Response Programming , 1977 .
[57] A. Vandierendonck,et al. The interaction between stop signal inhibition and distractor interference in the flanker and Stroop task. , 2004, Acta psychologica.
[58] John T. Wixted,et al. Methodology in experimental psychology , 2002 .
[59] J. Schall,et al. Performance monitoring by the supplementary eye ® eld , 2000 .
[60] M. Botvinick,et al. Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. , 1998, Science.
[61] Leanne Boucher,et al. Influence of history on saccade countermanding performance in humans and macaque monkeys , 2007, Vision Research.
[62] Robert F. Simons,et al. Oops!.. I did it again: An ERP and behavioral study of double-errors , 2008, Brain and Cognition.
[63] J. F. Corso,et al. Pitch-discrimination at high frequencies by air- and bone-conduction. , 1965, The American journal of psychology.
[64] E. Soetens,et al. Expectancy or Automatic Facilitation? Separating Sequential Effects in Two-Choice Reaction Time , 1985 .
[65] Douglas L. Medin,et al. Context theory of classification learning. , 1978 .