Post-stop-signal adjustments: inhibition improves subsequent inhibition.

Performance in the stop-signal paradigm involves a balance between going and stopping, and one way that this balance is struck is through shifting priority away from the go task, slowing responses after a stop signal, and improving the probability of inhibition. In 6 experiments, the authors tested whether there is a corresponding shift in priority toward the stop task, speeding reaction time to the stop signal. Consistent with this hypothesis, stop-signal reaction time (SSRT) decreased on the trial immediately following a stop signal in each experiment. Experiments 2-4 used 2 very different stop signals within a modality, and stopping improved when the stop stimulus repeated and alternated. Experiments 5 and 6 presented stop signals in different modalities and showed that SSRT improved only when the stop stimulus repeated within a modality. These results demonstrate within-modality post-stop-signal speeding of response inhibition.

[1]  Bernhard Hommel,et al.  Interactions between stimulus-stimulus congruence and stimulus-response compatibility , 1997 .

[2]  E. Tulving,et al.  Organization of memory. , 1973 .

[3]  Martina Rieger,et al.  Inhibitory after‐effects in the stop signal paradigm , 1999 .

[4]  Rajesh P. N. Rao,et al.  A rational decision making framework for inhibitory control , 2010, NIPS.

[5]  G. Logan,et al.  Impulsivity and Inhibitory Control , 1997 .

[6]  Marcel Brass,et al.  When doing nothing is an option: The neural correlates of deciding whether to act or not , 2009, NeuroImage.

[7]  G. Logan On the ability to inhibit thought and action , 1984 .

[8]  J. Schall,et al.  Executive control of countermanding saccades by the supplementary eye field , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[9]  T. Wager,et al.  Motivational influences on response inhibition measures. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  Darryl W. Schneider,et al.  Automatic and Controlled Response Inhibition: Associative Learning in the Go/no-go and Stop-signal Paradigms the Go/no-go Paradigm and the Stop-signal Paradigm , 2022 .

[11]  Elizabeth B. Liddle,et al.  Looking before you leap: A theory of motivated control of action , 2009, Cognition.

[12]  William M. Hartmann,et al.  Psychoacoustics: Facts and Models , 2001 .

[13]  W. Neill,et al.  Persistence of negative priming: II. Evidence for episodic trace retrieval. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[14]  L. Pessoa,et al.  Interactions between cognition and motivation during response inhibition , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  W. Neill,et al.  Persistence of negative priming: Steady state or decay? , 1992 .

[16]  Gordon D Logan,et al.  Evidence for an Error Monitoring Deficit in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder , 2004, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[17]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[18]  Patrick G. Bissett,et al.  Post-stop-signal slowing: strategies dominate reflexes and implicit learning. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[19]  W. Neill,et al.  Persistence of negative priming: II. Evidence for episodic trace retrieval , 1992 .

[20]  I. Franks,et al.  Predictability influences stopping and response control. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  T. Palmeri Exemplar similarity and the development of automaticity. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[22]  Donald Laming,et al.  Information theory of choice-reaction times , 1968 .

[23]  R. Nosofsky American Psychological Association, Inc. Choice, Similarity, and the Context Theory of Classification , 2022 .

[24]  T. K. Srull,et al.  Content and Process Specificity in the Effects of Prior Experiences , 1990 .

[25]  Frederick Verbruggen,et al.  Long-term aftereffects of response inhibition: memory retrieval, task goals, and cognitive control. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  H. Levitt Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. , 1971, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[27]  G. Logan Toward an instance theory of automatization. , 1988 .

[28]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  Performance Monitoring by the Anterior Cingulate Cortex During Saccade Countermanding , 2003, Science.

[29]  W. Notebaert,et al.  Effects of stimulus-stimulus compatibility and stimulus-response compatibility on response inhibition. , 2005, Acta psychologica.

[30]  Frederick Verbruggen,et al.  Short-term aftereffects of response inhibition: repetition priming or between-trial control adjustments? , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[31]  P. Rabbitt Errors and error correction in choice-response tasks. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[32]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and cognitive control. , 1975 .

[33]  Gordon D Logan,et al.  Reduced response readiness delays stop signal inhibition. , 2002, Acta psychologica.

[34]  R. Biddulph,et al.  Differential Pitch Sensitivity of the Ear , 1931 .

[35]  Russell A. Poldrack,et al.  Inhibition-related Activation in the Right Inferior Frontal Gyrus in the Absence of Inhibitory Cues , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[36]  E. Tulving,et al.  Episodic and semantic memory , 1972 .

[37]  Leanne Boucher,et al.  Nonindependent and nonstationary response times in stopping and stepping saccade tasks , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[38]  G. Logan Repetition priming and automaticity: Common underlying mechanisms? , 1990, Cognitive Psychology.

[39]  Patrick G. Bissett,et al.  Dissociating interference-control processes between memory and response. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[40]  Patrick G. Bissett,et al.  Balancing Cognitive Demands: Control Adjustments in the Stop-signal Paradigm Stop-signal Paradigm Post-stop-signal Slowing , 2011 .

[41]  Alan S. Brown,et al.  Information Processing and Cognition: The Loyola Symposium , 1976 .

[42]  B. Hommel,et al.  A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task , 2004, Psychological research.

[43]  D. Meyer,et al.  A Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1993 .

[44]  T. Zandt Analysis of Response Time Distributions , 2002 .

[45]  Robert S. Wyer,et al.  On the Indistinguishability of Exemplar Memory and Abstraction in Category Representation , 2014 .

[46]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Use of a delayed signal to stop a visual reaction-time response. , 1966 .

[47]  L. Barsalou On the indistinguishability of exemplar memory and abstraction in category representation , 1990 .

[48]  E. Awh,et al.  Conflict adaptation effects in the absence of executive control , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[49]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  Busy doing nothing: Evidence for nonaction-effect binding , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[50]  Hugo Fastl,et al.  Psychoacoustics: Facts and Models , 1990 .

[51]  W. Neill,et al.  Persistence of negative priming: Steady state or decay? , 1992 .

[52]  Erik M Altmann,et al.  Testing probability matching and episodic retrieval accounts of response repetition effects in task switching. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[53]  G. Logan Executive control of thought and action , 1985 .

[54]  KongFatt Wong-Lin,et al.  Optimal performance in a countermanding saccade task , 2010, Brain Research.

[55]  G. Logan,et al.  On the ability to inhibit thought and action: general and special theories of an act of control. , 2014, Psychological review.

[56]  Bryan Rodgers,et al.  What does a Man do after he Makes an Error? An Analysis of Response Programming , 1977 .

[57]  A. Vandierendonck,et al.  The interaction between stop signal inhibition and distractor interference in the flanker and Stroop task. , 2004, Acta psychologica.

[58]  John T. Wixted,et al.  Methodology in experimental psychology , 2002 .

[59]  J. Schall,et al.  Performance monitoring by the supplementary eye ® eld , 2000 .

[60]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. , 1998, Science.

[61]  Leanne Boucher,et al.  Influence of history on saccade countermanding performance in humans and macaque monkeys , 2007, Vision Research.

[62]  Robert F. Simons,et al.  Oops!.. I did it again: An ERP and behavioral study of double-errors , 2008, Brain and Cognition.

[63]  J. F. Corso,et al.  Pitch-discrimination at high frequencies by air- and bone-conduction. , 1965, The American journal of psychology.

[64]  E. Soetens,et al.  Expectancy or Automatic Facilitation? Separating Sequential Effects in Two-Choice Reaction Time , 1985 .

[65]  Douglas L. Medin,et al.  Context theory of classification learning. , 1978 .